Cognitive area The cognitive area sees behaviour as being heavily influenced by ones cognitive processes. The area likens human cognitive processes to.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Capacity vs. bottleneck theories
Advertisements

Perception: Attention Experiments Intro Psych Mar 3, 2010 Class #18.
Perception of syllable prominence by listeners with and without competence in the tested language Anders Eriksson 1, Esther Grabe 2 & Hartmut Traunmüller.
Chapter 1: Information and Computation. Cognitive Science  José Luis Bermúdez / Cambridge University Press 2010 Overview Review key ideas from last few.
Sentence Memory: A Constructive Versus Interpretive Approach Bransford, J.D., Barclay, J.R., & Franks, J.J.
Listening Tonja L. Root, Ed.D. Early Childhood & Reading Education Valdosta State University Valdosta, GA Root, T. (2008). Listening. Retrieved
ATTENTION Don Hine School of Psychology UNE Learning Objectives By the end of this lecture you should be able to: Define attention and describe 4 key.
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 3 – Attention July 8, 2003.
Attention Focus on what matters. What is Attention? Selection –Needed to avoid “information overload” –Related to Limited Capacity Concentration –Applying.
1 Pattern Recognition (cont.). 2 Auditory pattern recognition Stimuli for audition is alternating patterns of high and low air pressure called sound waves.
The Effects of Increased Cognitive Demands on the Written Discourse Ability of Young Adolescents Ashleigh Elaine Zumwalt Eastern Illinois University.
© 2001 Dr. Laura Snodgrass, Ph.D.1 Attention Determines which codes get processing Often associated with conscious awareness A continuum that varies with.
Cognitive Psychology. This unit is split into 4 aspects:  The nature of memory, including its stages, capacity, duration, encoding  Models of memory,
Pay Attention! Kimberley Clow
AQA Questions and answers
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 3 – Attention April 14, 2003.
Contents Cognitive Psychology What is Cognitive Psychology? Assumptions Methods of Investigation Core Studies from Cognitive Psychology - Loftus and Palmer.
Exam feedback ► 1(a) the type of memory strategy – whether Ps were instructed to form a mental image or whether they were instructed to memorise words.
Listening Tonja L. Root, Ed.D. Early Childhood & Reading Education Valdosta State University Valdosta, GA
Perception: Attention – Module 11 General Psych 1 March 1, 2005 Class #11.
Reliability of one cognitive process
MEMORY COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY PAPER 1 LEARNING OBJECTIVES By the end of the session you will be able to: –Describe what memory is –Outline (draw) the MSM.
Basic components of memory
DICHOTIC LISTENING In a picnic basket, she had peanut butter sandwiches and chocolate brownies…. cat, large, day, apple, friend, every, select, sandwiches.
What is attention? What are the effects of paying attention?
Duration is the amount of time a memory lasts in out short term memory. The duration of Short Term Memory lasts up to about seconds and occasionally.
Making A Case Interviewing Witnesses. MAKING A CASE Interviewing Witnesses Interviewing Suspects Creating A Profile Recognising Faces.
Listening Tonja L. Root, Ed.D. Early Childhood & Special Education Valdosta State University Valdosta, GA Root, T. (2012). Listening. Retrieved from.
Selective Attention & Spatial Attention Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 4/14 /2015: Lecture 03-2 This Powerpoint presentation.
Sperry Physiological Psychology The Core Studies.
Baron-Cohen Cognitive Psychology The Core Studies.
Extension: How could psychologists go about carrying out research? Discuss with someone else why you think psychologists carry out research.
Selective Attention
Strategies for Improving Concentration & Memory -Knowledge Zone.
Listening Tonja L. Root, Ed.D. Early Childhood & Reading Education Valdosta State University Valdosta, GA
AICE Psychology Introduction.
What now? Is this the best?
Structural, Phonological, Semantic
Aim To test Cherry’s findings on attention ‘more rigorously’. Sample
Cognitive Approach What are the main assumptions of the Cognitive Approach? Write these on the wallboards Use the pictures to help you You should know.
Assist. Prof. Dr. Ilmiye Seçer Fall
Research questions Daniel Simons and Christopher Chabris built on previous research from Neisser (1975) to investigate the nature of inattentional blindness.
Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages
Levels of Processing Memory Model (LoP)
What is the best environment to study in?
Unit 2 Chapter 7 Projects In Professional Communication
INTRODUCTION TO COMMUNITY PHARMACY
Memory.
Cognitive Processes PSY 334
Cognitive Approach Short-term memory – a limited amount of processing takes place here. Short term memory has a very limited capacity (7 items +/- 2)
Lesson 5. Lesson 5 Extraneous variables Extraneous variable (EV) is a general term for any variable, other than the IV, that might affect the results.
Attention as a Limited Capacity Resource
Objective = fact Subjective = opinion Objectivity
Evidence for the Working Memory Model.
Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction
Use of Models to explain behaviour
PSYA1: Cognitive Psychology Memory
Bartlett’s theory of Reconstructive Memory
The Wilcoxon signed rank test
Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction
Both of these experiments are investigating the effects of music on learning, specifically whether music can improve learning. Each experiment has been.
The cognitive area.
Interpersonal Communication
The Cognitive Perspective
Effects of Sexualization in Advertisements
Experiment 1: Aim, sample, method and procedure
Cognitive Area: Grant et al. (1998) study of Context-dependent memory
Shadowing Task Cherry, 1953 Attended Unattended
Presentation transcript:

Cognitive area The cognitive area sees behaviour as being heavily influenced by ones cognitive processes. The area likens human cognitive processes to a computer. In that, data is input, processed, stored, and retrieved. The area looks specifically at the internal processes of individuals alongside their thinking patterns. Main topics within this area include memory and attention. It is important to note we cannot see our thinking or memory. And so, we can only see the end result of these processes as displayed through behaviours. This means psychologists have to use a variety of techniques to study cognitive processes.

Moray (1959) Auditory attention Area: Cognitive Area Theme: Attention

Background On a daily basis, our brains are bombarded with information from all of our senses. Attention is the cognitive process that enables us to select some of this information to concentrate on, whilst rejecting (and ignoring) other information. Key term: Selective (or focussed) attention “the capacity to focus or process a certain stimuli selectively, when several other stimuli occur simultaneously”

Colin Cherry introduced “Cherry’s Cocktail Party Effect” Colin Cherry introduced “Cherry’s Cocktail Party Effect”. Cherry suggests if you hear your name in another conversation, your attention will be drawn from whatever you are focusing on and onto the conversation where your name was mentioned. This suggests the possibility to have selective/focussed attention. One way to measure an individuals selective attention is to use shadowing. This is where one message is fed into one ear, and another into the other. Another name for this is a dichotic listening task.

According to Cherry (1953), those who ‘shadowed’ a message in one ear, were unaware of the content in the other ear.

Method: Laboratory experiment Tape recorder with two amplifiers to allow two outputs – one to each ear through a set of headphones The messages were equal levels of loudness, judged by each participant Before each experiment the participants were given four passages of prose to shadow for practice All passages throughout the study were recorded by one male speaker Evaluate this:

Aims: Experiment 1: Moray aimed to test Cherry's findings – do individuals have selective/focused attention? Experiment 2 & 3: Moray aimed to investigate other factors that can affect attention in dichotic listening task.

Experiment 1 Method: Repeated measures design IV = dichotic listening task and recognition task DV = number of words recognised correctly in rejected message Sample = undergraduates, research workers, male and female. Number unknown

Experiment 1 Procedure: In experiment 1, a short list of simple words was repeatedly presented to one of the participants ears, whilst they shadowed a prose message presented to the other ear. The list was repeated 35 times. The participant was asked to recall the content of the rejected message (list of words). About 30 seconds later they were given a recognition test using similar material, yet not presented in the list nor the passage, as a control.

Experiment 1 Results: The mean number of words recognised (out of 7) Shadowed message (prose) 4.9 Rejected message (list of words) 1.9 Similar words from either 2.6 Overall, there was no trace of material from the rejected message being recognised. The difference between the new material and the shadowed message material was significant. The 30 second delay was unlikely to have caused forgetting, because words from early in the shadowed message were recognised.

Experiment 1 Conclusion: In a situation where a participant directs his attention to a message in one ear, and rejects a message in the other ear, almost none of the verbal content of the rejected message is able to penetrate the attention block.

Experiment 2 Method: Independent measures design IV = whether or not instructions were prefixed by participants own name DV = the number of affective instructions Sample = undergraduates, research workers, male and female. Total 12

Experiment 2 Procedure: Experiment 2 was conducted to find out the limits of attentional block. Participants shadowed ten short passages of light fiction. They were told that their responses would be recorded and that they should aim to make as little mistakes as possible. The individuals were told to ignore the rejected message. In the rejected message, Moray wanted to know would the participants attention go towards the rejected message if it included their name.

Experiment 2 Results: In affective condition (instructions with name) 20 out of 39 messages were heard In non-affective conditions (instructions without name) 4 out of 39 messages were heard (3 results were rejected as participants started paying attention to rejected messages). Results were significant.

Experiment 2 Conclusions: Subjectively 'important' messages, such as a person's own name, can penetrate the block.

Experiment 3 Method: Independent measures design IV = whether digits were inserted into one or two messages, and whether participants had to answer questions about the shadowed message at the end of each passage or just recall numbers DV = the number of digits correctly recalled Sample = undergraduates, research workers, male and female. Total 28 (two groups of 14)

Experiment 3 Procedure: In experiment 3, participants were required to shadow one of two simultaneous dichotic messages. In some of the messages digits were added towards the end of the message in either both messages or in one. The position of the numbers in the messages and whether they were relative to each other in the two messages varied. One group was told they'd be asked questions about the content of the shadowed message at the end. The other group as asked to remember as many numbers as they could.

Experiment 3 Conclusion: While perhaps not impossible, it is very difficult to make 'neutral' material important enough to break through the block set up in dichotic shadowing.