Filling-in afterimage colors between the lines

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Rob van Lier & Mark Vergeer Filling in the afterimage after the image BEST VISUAL ILLUSION CONTEST 2008 This presentation should be viewed in powerpoint.
Advertisements

Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs
Gemmata obscuriglobus
Olfaction: How Fish Catch a Whiff
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R447-R448 (June 2017)
Convergent Evolution: Gene Sharing by Eukaryotic Plant Pathogens
Decision Making: How the Brain Weighs the Evidence
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Microbiology: Mixing Wine, Chocolate, and Coffee
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Cell Biology: Microtubule Collisions to the Rescue
Sexual Selection: Roles Evolving
Insect Vision: A Neuron that Anticipates an Object’s Path
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Morphogens: Precise Outputs from a Variable Gradient
Infant cognition Current Biology
Alternative Strategies: The Evolution of Switch Points
Circadian clock determines the timing of rooster crowing
Phosphoinositide Function in Cytokinesis
Volume 23, Issue 23, Pages R1025-R1026 (December 2013)
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Antarctic sea ice losses drive gains in benthic carbon drawdown
Volume 19, Issue 21, Pages R971-R973 (November 2009)
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 19, Issue 6, Pages R233-R234 (March 2009)
Honeybee Communication: A Signal for Danger
Volume 23, Issue 7, Pages R265-R266 (April 2013)
The many colours of ‘the dress’
Fifty years of illumination about the natural levels of adaptation
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Time Perception: Space–Time in the Brain
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Carel ten Cate, Machteld N. Verzijden, Eric Etman  Current Biology 
Volume 24, Issue 7, Pages R262-R263 (March 2014)
Insect Vision: A Neuron that Anticipates an Object’s Path
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Elementary motion detectors
Figs and fig wasps Current Biology
Optic flow induces spatial filtering in fruit flies
It’s all about the constraints
Nonsomatotopic Organization of the Higher Motor Centers in Octopus
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic information in macaque inferior temporal cortex  Kasper Vinken, Rufin Vogels  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages R188-R189 (March 2015)
Janina Hesse, Susanne Schreiber  Current Biology 
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
FOXO transcription factors
Ben Vermaercke, Hans P. Op de Beeck  Current Biology 
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Color Constancy for an Unseen Surface
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1
Sensory Neurophysiology: Motion Vision during Motor Action
Basal bodies Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Volume 24, Issue 20, Pages R987-R988 (October 2014)
Presentation transcript:

Filling-in afterimage colors between the lines Rob van Lier, Mark Vergeer, Stuart Anstis  Current Biology  Volume 19, Issue 8, Pages R323-R324 (April 2009) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.010 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Matching afterimage colors. (A) The adapting stimulus was a colored star with four red and four cyan quadrilateral spikes and a grey inner area (presented for 1000 ms), which alternated over time with two successive test outlines (presented 500 ms each). The stimulus sequence induced differently colored afterimages within the test outlines. The observers had to match the afterimage color in the center of the first and the second test outline. Four basic stimulus conditions are considered: CiRo-I: Cyan inside, Red outside, First outline (which refers to the condition where the cyan spikes of the previously presented adapting stimulus appeared at a position inside, and the red spikes outside the first test outline); RiCo-I: Red inside, Cyan outside, First outline; CiRo-II: Cyan inside, Red outside, Second outline; RiCo-II: Red inside, Cyan outside, Second outline. (B) The color matching results (mean results for 3 observers) are plotted in CIE (x,y) color space. The small cross in the diagram on the left indicates the CIE (x,y) values of the background during the matching phase; the diagram on the right zooms in on that region. The arrows represent the mean color impression with respect to the background. Generally, the RiCo conditions revealed cyanish afterimages, whereas the CiRo conditions revealed reddish afterimages. For each of the observers, the mean color impression was different from the background in each condition (F(2,6) > 18.2; p < 0.005), and the RiCo conditions differed from the CiRo conditions (F(2,13) > 73.8; p < 0.0001). See Supplemental Data (Experiment 1) for detailed results. Current Biology 2009 19, R323-R324DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.010) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions