Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Advertisements

Sea turtles Current Biology
Animal Cognition: Monkeys Pass the Mirror Test
Behavioral Neuroscience: Crawling Is a No-Brainer for Fruit Fly Larvae
Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Convergent Evolution: Gene Sharing by Eukaryotic Plant Pathogens
Matthew Collett, Paul Graham, Thomas S. Collett  Current Biology 
Volume 14, Issue 6, Pages R221-R224 (March 2004)
Social Learning: The Importance of Copying Others
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Liangtang Chang, Qin Fang, Shikun Zhang, Mu-ming Poo, Neng Gong 
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Matthew Collett, Thomas S. Collett, Mandyam V. Srinivasan 
Idiothetic Path Integration in the Fruit Fly Drosophila melanogaster
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Navigation: Whence Our Sense of Direction?
Cell Division: Experiments and Modelling Unite to Resolve the Middle
Sexual Selection: Roles Evolving
Meiosis: Organizing Microtubule Organizers
Insect Neurobiology: An Eye to Forward Motion
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Behavioural Genetics: Evolutionary Fingerprint of the ‘Invisible Hand’
Behavior: Warriors Shaking Hands
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Animal Behavior: The Truman Show for Ants
Volume 17, Issue 16, Pages R650-R652 (August 2007)
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation
Jes Rust, Torsten Wappler  Current Biology 
Unearthing the Phylogenetic Roots of Sleep
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Ecology: The Tropical Deforestation Debt
Volume 25, Issue 13, Pages (June 2015)
Volume 27, Issue 5, Pages (March 2017)
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
Homing Ants Get Confused When Nest Cues Are Also Route Cues
Sea turtles Current Biology
Cornelia Buehlmann, Paul Graham, Bill S. Hansson, Markus Knaden 
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Elementary motion detectors
Thomas S Collett, Paul Graham  Current Biology 
Animal Behaviour: Feeding the Superorganism
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Group Behaviour: Leadership by Those in Need
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Sensorimotor Neuroscience: Motor Precision Meets Vision
Insect Navigation: How Do Wasps Get Home?
Volume 18, Issue 15, Pages R641-R645 (August 2008)
Small RNAs: How Seeds Remember To Obey Their Mother
Higher-Order Figure Discrimination in Fly and Human Vision
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Social Learning: Ants and the Meaning of Teaching
How Navigational Guidance Systems Are Combined in a Desert Ant
Basal bodies Current Biology
Calibration of vector navigation in desert ants
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Endosperm Imprinting: A Child Custody Battle?
Liangtang Chang, Qin Fang, Shikun Zhang, Mu-ming Poo, Neng Gong 
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack Basil el Jundi  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 15, Pages R748-R750 (August 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.051 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 The fly’s local search behavior compared to the ant’s foraging trips. (A) Schematic drawing of the trajectory of the fly’s local search behavior. After finding food, a hungry fly loops around it (orange track). (B) Drawing of an ant’s path during a foraging trip. After a complicated foraging bout, the ant returns to its nest in a straight line (red trajectory). Arrows indicate the path-integrator vectors that contain direction and distance information to the nest. (C) Schematic drawing of the experiments from [3]. The flies’ local searches after discovering food (orange trajectories) were tested. Under all shown conditions, when visual, olfactory, and pheromonal cues were eliminated, respectively, the flies still exhibited a local search behavior. (D) Drawing of the displacement experiment from [3]. Despite olfactory cues that indicated the location of the food source in the arena’s perimeter, after transfer of the food, flies looped around the fictive food spot. (E) Summary of the fly’s (left) and ant’s (right) path integrator used for different behaviors: During their local searches, flies rotated around the shorter angular path to face the food drop (dark green arrow). The longer the distance to the origin prior to the turn the longer flies walk after the turn (red track), similar to the behavior of a homing insect. Black arrows indicate the path-integrator vectors. Current Biology 2017 27, R748-R750DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.051) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions