Heritability of the Specific Cognitive Ability of Face Perception

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Associations Among Adolescent Conduct Problems and Perceived Peer and Parental Acceptance of Adolescent Alcohol Use Julia D. Grant, Kathleen K. Bucholz,
Advertisements

ACDE model and estimability Why can’t we estimate (co)variances due to A, C, D and E simultaneously in a standard twin design?
Two views of brain function Marcus E. Raichle Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 14, Issue 4, Pages (April 2010) DOI: /j.tics
Biological Approaches to Global Environment Change Mitigation and Remediation F. Ian Woodward, Richard D. Bardgett, John A. Raven, Alistair M. Hetherington.
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine
Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci ;57(1): doi: /iovs Figure Legend:
Aimee S. Dunlap, Matthew E. Nielsen, Anna Dornhaus, Daniel R. Papaj 
Key research: Van Leeuwen et al
Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs
Motor Task Variation Induces Structural Learning
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Etiologic features of newly diagnosed epilepsy: Hospital-based study of 892 consecutive patients in West China  Yang Si, Ling Liu, Jia Hu, Jie Mu, Jia-jia.
Obesity: Cognitive Impairment and the Failure to ‘Eat Right’
Volume 16, Issue 2, Pages (January 2006)
Ocean Depths: The Mesopelagic and Implications for Global Warming
Victoria F. Ratcliffe, David Reby  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 20, Pages (October 2015)
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
CHEK2*1100delC and Susceptibility to Breast Cancer: A Collaborative Analysis Involving 10,860 Breast Cancer Cases and 9,065 Controls from 10 Studies 
Tzvi Ganel, Eran Chajut, Daniel Algom  Current Biology 
Morphogens: Precise Outputs from a Variable Gradient
Infant cognition Current Biology
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Encoding of Motor Skill in the Corticomuscular System of Musicians
The Primate Cerebellum Selectively Encodes Unexpected Self-Motion
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Marianne Elias, Colin Fontaine, F.J. Frank van Veen  Current Biology 
Margarita V. Chibalina, Dmitry A. Filatov  Current Biology 
Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Elephant cognition Current Biology
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
Volume 22, Issue 13, Pages (July 2012)
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Elementary motion detectors
Face Perception Neuron
Volume 23, Issue 2, Pages (January 2013)
Noa Raz, Ella Striem, Golan Pundak, Tanya Orlov, Ehud Zohary 
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Power Calculation for QTL Association
Humans Have an Expectation That Gaze Is Directed Toward Them
IgE in unselected like-sexed monozygotic and dizygotic twins at birth and at 6 to 9 years of age: High but dissimilar genetic influence on IgE levels 
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Self-Control in Chimpanzees Relates to General Intelligence
Category Selectivity in the Ventral Visual Pathway Confers Robustness to Clutter and Diverted Attention  Leila Reddy, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Knowledgeable Lemurs Become More Central in Social Networks
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Age-Related Declines of Stability in Visual Perceptual Learning
Expression Partitioning between Genes Duplicated by Polyploidy under Abiotic Stress and during Organ Development  Zhenlan Liu, Keith L. Adams  Current.
Jenni Deveau, Daniel J. Ozer, Aaron R. Seitz  Current Biology 
Evelina Fedorenko, John Duncan, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Temporal coordination signals coalition quality
Grasping Weber's law Current Biology
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages R947-R948 (December 2011)
Basal bodies Current Biology
Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception
Animal Cognition: Aesop's Fable Flies from Fiction to Fact
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Genetic and environmental contributions to abdominal aortic aneurysm development in a twin population  Carl Magnus Wahlgren, MD, PhD, Emma Larsson, MD,
Volume 24, Issue 20, Pages R987-R988 (October 2014)
Presentation transcript:

Heritability of the Specific Cognitive Ability of Face Perception Qi Zhu, Yiying Song, Siyuan Hu, Xiaobai Li, Moqian Tian, Zonglei Zhen, Qi Dong, Nancy Kanwisher, Jia Liu  Current Biology  Volume 20, Issue 2, Pages 137-142 (January 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.067 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Intraclass Correlations for Monozygotic and Dizygotic Twins (A) The old/new recognition task. The y axis shows the intraclass correlations (r) in face-specific recognition ability (FRA) for monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins. (B) Face-inversion effect (FIE). (C) Composite-face effect (CFE). (D) Global-to-local interference (GLI). Current Biology 2010 20, 137-142DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.067) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Heritability of Face Processing and Its Relation to General Cognitive Ability (A) Estimates of proportions of variance due to genetic and environmental influences, as derived from maximum-likelihood model-fitting analyses of twins. For each measure, the full model fit well, suggested by both chi squares (FRA: 2.29; FIE: 2.17; CFE: 7.69; reaction time [RT]: 2.70) and Akaike fit indices (FRA: −3.71; FIE: −3.83; CFE: 1.69; RT: −3.30). For each measure, the best-fitting model was one that included only nonadditive genetic and nonshared environment parameters, with the following chi squares with four degrees of freedom: 2.29, 2.17, 7.69, and 2.70, respectively. In other words, dropping the additive genetic component of variance from the full model did not significantly reduce the fit of the model (Δχ2(1) = 0). In contrast, dropping either the nonadditive genetic parameter (E model, likelihood-ratio test, FRA: Δχ2(1) = 14.71, p < 0.001; FIE: Δχ2(1) = 6.57, p < 0.01; CFE: Δχ2(1) = 8.58, p < 0.005; RT: Δχ2(1) = 14.66, p < 0.001) or nonshared environment parameter (D model, p values < 0.0001) from the model significantly worsened the fit. (B) Correlation between IQ (assessed by Raven's advanced progressive matrices [APM]) (36 items, mean raw score = 25.8, standard deviation [SD] = 4.03) and face perception ability measured by the face-inversion effect (FIE) (left) and the composite-face effect (CFE) (middle). Correlation between FIE and CFE is shown (right). (C) Genetic influence on face recognition increases with age. The data from the childhood group (age 7–12, age mean = 10.8, SD = 1.23) and the adolescence group (age 13–19, age mean = 14.9, SD = 1.63) were simultaneously analyzed in one model with different parameter estimates for the two age groups. The genetic effects were significantly larger for the adolescence group than for the childhood group, because forcing parameter estimates to be equal for the two age groups significantly worsened the fit of the model (FRA: Δχ2(2) = 11.83, p < 0.005; FIE: Δχ2(2) = 12.19, p < 0.005; CFE: Δχ2(2) = 6.25, p < 0.05; and RT: Δχ2(2) = 8.81, p < 0.05). Current Biology 2010 20, 137-142DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.067) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions