Cécile Fruteau, Eric van Damme, Ronald Noë  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Mushroom-Body Memories: An Obituary Prematurely Written? Ronald L. Davis, Martin Giurfa Current Biology Volume 22, Issue 8, Pages R272-R275 (April 2012)
Advertisements

Erman Misirlisoy, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
A Multisensory Network for Olfactory Processing
The Dynamic Range of Human Lightness Perception
Germán Sumbre, Graziano Fiorito, Tamar Flash, Binyamin Hochner 
Animal Cognition: How Archer Fish Learn to Down Rapidly Moving Targets
Victoria F. Ratcliffe, David Reby  Current Biology 
Coldness Triggers Northward Flight in Remigrant Monarch Butterflies
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Pattern and Component Motion Responses in Mouse Visual Cortical Areas
Sizing up dogs Current Biology
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset Monkeys
Cell Division: Experiments and Modelling Unite to Resolve the Middle
Chimpanzees Trust Their Friends
Sergei Gepshtein, Martin S. Banks  Current Biology 
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Volume 26, Issue 8, Pages (April 2016)
First-Pass Processing of Value Cues in the Ventral Visual Pathway
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R450-R452 (June 2017)
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Margarita V. Chibalina, Dmitry A. Filatov  Current Biology 
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Kensaku Nomoto, Susana Q. Lima  Current Biology 
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Publication metrics and success on the academic job market
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 22, Issue 20, Pages (October 2012)
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Kyoko Yoshida, Nobuhito Saito, Atsushi Iriki, Masaki Isoda 
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Positive selection on gene expression in the human brain
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Pattern and Component Motion Responses in Mouse Visual Cortical Areas
Volume 26, Issue 5, Pages (March 2016)
Asif A. Ghazanfar, Daniel Y. Takahashi, Neil Mathur, W. Tecumseh Fitch 
Direct Two-Dimensional Access to the Spatial Location of Covert Attention in Macaque Prefrontal Cortex  Elaine Astrand, Claire Wardak, Pierre Baraduc,
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Wild Vervet Monkeys Trade Tolerance and Specific Coalitionary Support for Grooming in Experimentally Induced Conflicts  Christèle Borgeaud, Redouan Bshary 
Volume 24, Issue 14, Pages (July 2014)
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Shade coffee farms promote genetic diversity of native trees
Volume 26, Issue 11, Pages (June 2016)
Auditory-Visual Crossmodal Integration in Perception of Face Gender
Cognitive neural prosthetics
Computer Use Changes Generalization of Movement Learning
Volume 18, Issue 17, Pages R728-R729 (September 2008)
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages (March 2008)
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces  Taiyong Bi, Juan Chen, Tiangang Zhou, Yong.
When Correlation Implies Causation in Multisensory Integration
Social Coordination: Patience Is a Virtue for Vervet Monkeys
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
Rank influences human sex differences in dyadic cooperation
The Geography of Ecological Niche Evolution in Mammals
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Responsibility Assignment in Redundant Systems
Presentation transcript:

Vervet Monkeys Solve a Multiplayer “Forbidden Circle Game” by Queuing to Learn Restraint  Cécile Fruteau, Eric van Damme, Ronald Noë  Current Biology  Volume 23, Issue 8, Pages 665-670 (April 2013) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.039 Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2013 23, 665-670DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.039) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Correlations between the Time Dominants Went beyond the Critical Distance and the Moment the Provider Opened the Container x axis represents trial number; y axis represents time elapsed after start of trial. The black line represents the time the last animal dominant to the provider left the 10 m radius circle around the container. The filled colored line represents the time the container was opened. Spearman rank correlations between “forbidden circle left” and “container opened”: Strasbourg: ρ = 0.999, p < 0.01; Donga: ρ = 0.830, p < 0.01; Picnic: ρ = 0.837, p < 0.01. Current Biology 2013 23, 665-670DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.039) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Cumulative Individual Learning Curves Cumulative time elapsed before each animal left the forbidden circle. The idealized curve of a hypothetical dominant is shown in (A). These leaving curves are closely correlated to the Weibull curves designated by the function R=A(1−2)−(x/L)S, where R is the cumulative retreat time; A is the asymptotic level (reached when the animal consistently shows restraint from the start of the trial); L (indicated by arrows) is the breakpoint (trial corresponding with A/2), the first trial after which the animal left the forbidden circle significantly faster than before; and s describes the steepness of the slope. Average z values of Kolmogorov-Smirnov goodness-of-fit tests between empirically observed and Weibull curves were 2.158 (Strasbourg), 3.419 (Donga), and 2.314 (Picnic). The vertical lines in (C) and (D) indicate a break of >6 months between two series of trials in the Loskop groups. M = male; F = female; numbers indicate rank, with 1 = highest rank. Current Biology 2013 23, 665-670DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.039) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Description of Each Dominant’s Time of Departure from the Forbidden Circle Lower, filled lines: time needed to leave the forbidden circle per individual. Upper lines: time at which the provider opened the container (blue, Strasbourg; green, Donga; orange, Picnic). For each group, the animals are ordered according to the trial at which they reached their breakpoint L (see Figure 2). Note that each time a dominant learned restraint, the provider temporarily took less time, but they relapsed to longer opening times when the next dominant started monopolizing. M = male; F = female; numbers indicate rank, with 1 = highest rank. For the Donga and Picnic groups, the dashed vertical line represents the >6-month break between trial blocks 1 and 2. Current Biology 2013 23, 665-670DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.039) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions