Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Reduced GABAergic Action in the Autistic Brain
Advertisements

Visual Influences on Echo Suppression
The Dynamic Range of Human Lightness Perception
Long-Term Speeding in Perceptual Switches Mediated by Attention-Dependent Plasticity in Cortical Visual Processing  Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky 
Neuronal Correlates of Metacognition in Primate Frontal Cortex
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky  Neuron 
Natalia Zaretskaya, Andreas Bartels  Current Biology 
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Pattern and Component Motion Responses in Mouse Visual Cortical Areas
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Volume 36, Issue 5, Pages (December 2002)
How Much the Eye Tells the Brain
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Sergei Gepshtein, Martin S. Banks  Current Biology 
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
Benjamin Thompson, Behzad Mansouri, Lisa Koski, Robert F. Hess 
Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness in Human Extrastriate Cortex
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky  Neuron 
Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking Activity in Early Visual Cortex
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Spike Timing-Dependent LTP/LTD Mediates Visual Experience-Dependent Plasticity in a Developing Retinotectal System  Yangling Mu, Mu-ming Poo  Neuron 
Volume 18, Issue 24, Pages (December 2008)
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Binocular Rivalry Requires Visual Attention
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons
Pattern and Component Motion Responses in Mouse Visual Cortical Areas
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Optic flow induces spatial filtering in fruit flies
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Jingping P. Xu, Zijiang J. He, Teng Leng Ooi  Current Biology 
The Normalization Model of Attention
David Pitcher, Vincent Walsh, Galit Yovel, Bradley Duchaine 
Martijn Barendregt, Ben M. Harvey, Bas Rokers, Serge O. Dumoulin 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages (March 2008)
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Tuning to Natural Stimulus Dynamics in Primary Auditory Cortex
Category Selectivity in the Ventral Visual Pathway Confers Robustness to Clutter and Diverted Attention  Leila Reddy, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
When Correlation Implies Causation in Multisensory Integration
Binocular Vision: The Eyes Add and Subtract
Kristy A. Sundberg, Jude F. Mitchell, John H. Reynolds  Neuron 
Claudia Lunghi, Uzay E. Emir, Maria Concetta Morrone, Holly Bridge 
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
The Perception and Misperception of Specular Surface Reflectance
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, Stefano Panzeri  Current Biology 
Volume 22, Issue 5, Pages (March 2012)
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Motion Adaptation and the Velocity Coding of Natural Scenes
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision P. Christiaan Klink, Jan W. Brascamp, Randolph Blake, Richard J.A. van Wezel  Current Biology  Volume 20, Issue 16, Pages 1464-1469 (August 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2010 20, 1464-1469DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Dynamics of Perceptual Exclusivity (A) Perceptual experiences during binocular rivalry. Exclusive percepts correspond entirely to one eye's stimulus. Mixed percepts resemble patch-like (piecemeal) or transparent superimpositions of the stimuli. (B) The average proportion of exclusivity for five observers, plotted against time and normalized by individual baselines, determined in four rivalry trials directly preceding the experiment. In the recovery stage, observers experienced normal binocular vision (black circles), monocular vision only (white squares), or no visual stimulation at all (gray asterisks). (C) The average epoch durations for mixed (left panel, black circles) and exclusive “left” and “right” percepts (right panel, white and black squares, respectively) of the both-eyes-stimulated condition. Dashed lines represent baseline levels. Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM). Current Biology 2010 20, 1464-1469DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Prerequisites for Decreasing Exclusivity (A) The average proportion of exclusive grating percepts over time for five observers. The eye-stimulus configuration was the same for most trials (“majority trials,” black circles) but was switched in some interleaved trials (“opposite configuration trials,” white squares). (B) Similar to (A), but here the monocular images were complex pictures of a house and a face, not orthogonal sinusoidal gratings. Dashed lines represent baseline exclusivity; ∗p < 0.05; error bars represent SEM. Current Biology 2010 20, 1464-1469DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Prerequisites for Recovery of Decreased Exclusivity (A) The plaid stimuli that were interleaved with rivalry trials. Matching plaids had the same components as the rivaling gratings, whereas nonmatching plaids' components had different spatial frequencies and orientations. (B) The average proportion of normalized exclusivity over time for five observers. Rivalry trials were interleaved with plaid presentations (gray bands). Matching plaids were presented simultaneously to both eyes (black circles) or one eye (gray asterisks). Nonmatching plaids were always presented to two eyes (white squares). The dashed line represents baseline exclusivity. (C) Exclusivity, compared between rivalry trials that directly preceded (white bars) and followed (gray bars) plaid presentation. ∗p < 0.05; error bars represent SEM. Current Biology 2010 20, 1464-1469DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 The Effect of Monocular Replay Rivalry (A) Whereas during rivalry, both eyes are simultaneously stimulated with conflicting images, replay rivalry consists of alternating monocular stimulations with a temporal structure based on individual perceptual reports during real rivalry. (B) The average proportion of normalized exclusivity in three rivalry trials that were each preceded by two replay trials (gray bands). The dashed line represents baseline exclusivity; ∗p < 0.05; error bars represent SEM. Current Biology 2010 20, 1464-1469DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.057) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions