S.A.R. Nouraei, N. de Pennington, J.G. Jones, R.H.S. Carpenter 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Piercing of Consciousness as a Threshold-Crossing Operation
Advertisements

Bram-Ernst Verhoef, Rufin Vogels, Peter Janssen  Neuron 
One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIP
Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes
Bram-Ernst Verhoef, Rufin Vogels, Peter Janssen  Neuron 
Randomized controlled trial comparing the McGrath videolaryngoscope with the C-MAC videolaryngoscope in intubating adult patients with potential difficult.
Volume 51, Issue 6, Pages (September 2006)
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Braden A. Purcell, Roozbeh Kiani  Neuron 
Sheng Li, Stephen D. Mayhew, Zoe Kourtzi  Neuron 
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Volume 93, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Wide inter-individual variability of bispectral index and spectral entropy at loss of consciousness during increasing concentrations of dexmedetomidine,
Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Hannah M. Bayer, Paul W. Glimcher  Neuron 
Volume 111, Issue 2, Pages (July 2016)
Co-administration of dexamethasone with peripheral nerve block: intravenous vs perineural application: systematic review, meta-analysis, meta-regression.
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Spatial Coding of the Predicted Impact Location of a Looming Object
Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Comparison of pattern of breathing with other measures of induction of anaesthesia, using propofol, methohexital, and sevoflurane†  T.L. Strickland, G.B.
Hyoung F. Kim, Hidetoshi Amita, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
R.W.D. Nickalls, W.W. Mapleson  British Journal of Anaesthesia 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Spatial Coding of the Predicted Impact Location of a Looming Object
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Jeremy B. Wilmer, Ken Nakayama  Neuron 
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
Eye Movement Preparation Modulates Neuronal Responses in Area V4 When Dissociated from Attentional Demands  Nicholas A. Steinmetz, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff
Colocalization of Multiple DNA Loci: A Physical Mechanism
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Volume 89, Issue 6, Pages (March 2016)
Effect of continuous low-dose intravenous diltiazem on epidural fentanyl analgesia after lower abdominal surgery  K. Nitahara, M. Matsunaga, K. Katori,
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Volume 75, Issue 5, Pages (September 2012)
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
B. Allaouchiche, F. Duflo, J.-P. Tournadre, R. Debon, D. Chassard 
Expertise in practice: an ethnographic study exploring acquisition and use of knowledge in anaesthesia  A. Smith, D. Goodwin, M. Mort, C. Pope  British.
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes
Anaesthesia for awake craniotomy—evolution of a technique that facilitates awake neurological testing  A. Sarang, J. Dinsmore  British Journal of Anaesthesia 
Comparison of 1% and 2% lidocaine epidural anaesthesia combined with sevoflurane general anaesthesia utilizing a constant bispectral index  A Shono, S.
Comparison of four strategies to reduce the pain associated with intravenous administration of rocuronium  A.B. Chiarella, D.T. Jolly, C.M. Huston, A.S.
Use of L’Abbé and pooled calibration plots to assess the relationship between severity of illness and effectiveness in studies of corticosteroids for.
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Systemic lidocaine fails to improve postoperative morphine consumption, postoperative recovery and quality of life in patients undergoing posterior spinal.
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Extreme value theory applied to postoperative breathing patterns
Volume 75, Issue 5, Pages (September 2012)
J.G. Laffey, é. Tobin, J.F. Boylan, A.J. McShane 
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
T. Suzuki, K. Iwasaki, N. Fukano, S. Hariya, S. Saeki, S. Ogawa 
Behavioral task and performance.
Social status gates social attention in monkeys
Optimizing Visual Motion Perception during Eye Movements
Valerio Mante, Vincent Bonin, Matteo Carandini  Neuron 
Maxwell H. Turner, Fred Rieke  Neuron 
Fig 1 Iso‐MAC chart for isoflurane (age ≥1 yr)
Presentation transcript:

Dose-related effect of sevoflurane sedation on higher control of eye movements and decision making  S.A.R. Nouraei, N. de Pennington, J.G. Jones, R.H.S. Carpenter  British Journal of Anaesthesia  Volume 91, Issue 2, Pages 175-183 (August 2003) DOI: 10.1093/bja/aeg158 Copyright © 2003 British Journal of Anaesthesia Terms and Conditions

Fig 1 The countermanding paradigm. In a control trial (top), at the start of the trial a fixation target (black square) is presented. The dotted circle represents the position of the subject's gaze. After a random delay, the target jumps unpredictably to the left or right, and the subject makes a saccade to track it after a delay, the saccadic latency. In a stop trial (below), fixation is followed by a target jump as in a control trial, but the fixation light reappears after a fixed period (the stop-signal delay), signalling to the subject that no saccade should be made. On some trials subjects successfully inhibit the saccade, on others they do not. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2003 91, 175-183DOI: (10.1093/bja/aeg158) Copyright © 2003 British Journal of Anaesthesia Terms and Conditions

Fig 2 Typical reciprobit plot of saccadic latency data from one subject (A) for control trials at four different sevoflurane doses. Cumulative histograms (i.e. plots of the proportion of trials on which a saccade has occurred by a particular time) are plotted on a probit scale as a function of reciprocal latency (using a reciprocal scale labelled for ease of interpretation with latencies rather than their reciprocals: longer latencies lie to the right). If the LATER model is correct, the data should then lie on a straight line. The lines are best-fit straight lines, subject to the constraint of parallelism. Note that the use of a probit scale tends to throw the tails of the distribution into prominence: the deviations at longer latencies in some cases are not statistically significant. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2003 91, 175-183DOI: (10.1093/bja/aeg158) Copyright © 2003 British Journal of Anaesthesia Terms and Conditions

Fig 3 Reciprocal median simple latency (±1 se) plotted as a function of sevoflurane dose (MAC) for each subject, with fitted regression lines. Note that the latency axes are chosen to be appropriate for each subject. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2003 91, 175-183DOI: (10.1093/bja/aeg158) Copyright © 2003 British Journal of Anaesthesia Terms and Conditions

Fig 4 Reciprocal median stop-signal reaction time (±1 se) plotted as a function of sevoflurane dose (MAC) for each subject, with fitted regression lines. Note that the SSRT axes are chosen to be appropriate for each subject. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2003 91, 175-183DOI: (10.1093/bja/aeg158) Copyright © 2003 British Journal of Anaesthesia Terms and Conditions

Fig 5 The LATER model. When reaction times are plotted as cumulative distributions on a probit scale as a function of the reciprocal of latency (a reciprobit plot), in general they are found to yield a straight line (a), implying that the reciprocal of latency is distributed as a Gaussian. The line intersects the P=50% ordinate at the median latency, and the t=infinity axis at a point I. This behaviour can be explained by a simple mechanism (the LATER model) in which a decision signal, S, initially at a level S0, rises linearly on presentation of a stimulus until it reaches a threshold, ST, at which point a response is initiated. On different trials, r varies randomly about a mean μ in a Gaussian manner, such that the distribution of latency is skewed (shaded area) and the reciprocal of latency is Gaussian, as observed. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2003 91, 175-183DOI: (10.1093/bja/aeg158) Copyright © 2003 British Journal of Anaesthesia Terms and Conditions