Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Advertisements

Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Volume 84, Issue 1, Pages (October 2014)
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Volume 24, Issue 13, Pages (July 2014)
José Vergara, Natsuko Rivera, Román Rossi-Pool, Ranulfo Romo  Neuron 
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Jennifer L. Hoy, Iryna Yavorska, Michael Wehr, Cristopher M. Niell 
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration
Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Dynamics of Eye-Position Signals in the Dorsal Visual System
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Daniel E. Winkowski, Eric I. Knudsen  Neuron 
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
The Normalization Model of Attention
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Posterior Parietal Cortex Encodes Autonomously Selected Motor Plans
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality
Computer Use Changes Generalization of Movement Learning
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Supervised Calibration Relies on the Multisensory Percept
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Optimizing Visual Motion Perception during Eye Movements
Volume 22, Issue 5, Pages (March 2012)
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages (November 2006)
Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Presentation transcript:

The Cerebellum Optimizes Perceptual Predictions about External Sensory Events  Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology  Volume 23, Issue 10, Pages 930-935 (May 2013) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027 Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2013 23, 930-935DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Task Design (A) A visual target (red bar) moved from the left side of the stimulus screen to the right. In the middle of its movement trajectory (middle panel), it disappeared under an opaque occluder. The speed of the target (∼16°/s) was always constant when the target was visible. However, the time the target needed to pass the occluder was varied experimentally. After the target had reappeared at the right end of the occluder, subjects had to manually indicate whether the target had reappeared “too early” or “too late,” given its constant speed. (B) The time of target reappearance Δt was varied relative to the duration the target would need to pass the occluder when moving at constant speed (i.e., 200 ms, middle target trajectory). Negative Δt (left exemplary trajectory) denotes that the target reappeared earlier as compared to a continuous movement with constant speed, whereas positive Δt (right trajectory) denotes that the target reappeared later. Current Biology 2013 23, 930-935DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Results of an Exemplary Control Subject (A) In the baseline experiment, the levels of Δt were equally distributed across trials, ranging between ±180 ms, as shown in the upper panel. Triangles pointing upward represent “too late” responses; triangles pointing downward represent “too early” responses. The lower panel exhibits the corresponding probability of “too late” responses for a given Δt. The fit of the psychometric function indicates a baseline point of subjective equivalence (bPSE at Ptoo late = 0.5) of Δt = 54 ms and a just noticeable difference (JND) of Δt = 52 ms. (B) In the recalibration experiment, the target reappeared 100 ms later than the bPSE (i.e., Δt = 154 ms for this subject) in 50% of trials, namely in the so-called recalibration trials (dark gray triangles). The remaining 50% of randomly interleaved probe trials were used to calculate the actual point of subjective equivalence (recalibration point of subjective equivalence; rPSE). Eighty percent of these probe trials were PEST trials (light gray triangles) in which Δt was governed by an adaptive staircase procedure (PEST) that approximated the PSE. Twenty percent of probe trials were “easy” trials (brown triangles) with Δts above the perceptual threshold. Recalibration trials induced a rightward shift of the corresponding psychometric function, resulting in an rPSE of 176 ms (lower panel). For further information on the different trial types and the calculation of the psychometric function, see Supplemental Experimental Procedures. For all individual subjects’ PSEs, see Figure S1. Current Biology 2013 23, 930-935DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Group Results of Patients and Controls (A) The baseline point of subjective equivalence (bPSE) was not significantly different between groups, although patients exhibited a slightly larger bPSE (mean: controls 30 ms, patients 57 ms). Error bars indicate ±SEM. (B) The just noticeable difference (JND), as a measure of perceptual accuracy, did not show a significant difference between both groups (median 55 ms in both groups). This indicates that both groups were equally able to form accurate sensory predictions. Error bars indicate the interquartile range. (C) Recalibration, i.e., the difference between rPSE and bPSE, showed a highly significant difference (p = 0.002) between groups, indicating an impairment of cerebellar patients in recalibrating their sensory predictions (mean: controls 120 ms, patients 52 ms). Error bars indicate ±SEM. For group results after exclusion of possible outliers, see Figure S2. Current Biology 2013 23, 930-935DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Comparison of Eye Movements between Patients and Controls To rule out that the observed difference in recalibration was due to eye movements, we tested for group differences in horizontal eye position, eye velocity, and the number of fixational saccades, respectively. Specifically, we ran ANOVAs with the factors group and experiment (and time bin). There were no significant group effects or interactions (p ≥ 0.05; the respective significance of each main factor is depicted in each panel; n.s. = not significant). This is further supported by the box plots for the three eye movement parameters shown here. There are no differences between patients (light gray) and controls (dark gray). The central red bars reflect the median of each measure, the vertical extent of each box captures the range between the 25th (Q1) and the 75th (Q3) percentile, and the whiskers extend to extreme values [except putative outliers “+,” i.e., values > Q3 + 1.5 (Q3 − Q1) or < Q1 − 1.5 (Q3 − Q1)]. Note that putative outliers were not excluded from statistical comparisons. See also Figure S3. Current Biology 2013 23, 930-935DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions