David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Erroneous quadruped walking depictions in natural history museums
Advertisements

Animal Vision: Rats Watch the Sky
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Theta Rhythm: Temporal Glue for Episodic Memory
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Comparative Cognition: Action Imitation Using Episodic Memory
Sizing up dogs Current Biology
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
A new galloping gait in an insect
Infant cognition Current Biology
G. Lorimer Moseley, Timothy J. Parsons, Charles Spence  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 24, Pages R1156-R1158 (December 2015)
Growth: A Model for Establishing Cell Size and Shape
Visual System: Prostriata — A Visual Area Off the Beaten Path
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Jes Rust, Torsten Wappler  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 23, Pages R1114-R1116 (December 2015)
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Yukiyasu Kamitani, Frank Tong  Current Biology 
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
Ecology: The Tropical Deforestation Debt
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Publication metrics and success on the academic job market
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Brain Imaging: Decoding Your Memories
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Time Perception: Space–Time in the Brain
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Volume 27, Issue 20, Pages R1106-R1107 (October 2017)
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
Climate Change: A Hybrid Zone Moves North
Robert A. Harris, Paul Graham, Thomas S. Collett  Current Biology 
Episodic Memory in Nonhuman Animals
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
The sound of change: visually-induced auditory synesthesia
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Evidence of episodic-like memory in cuttlefish
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages R784-R785 (September 2012)
Motor Control: No Constant but Change
Volume 16, Issue 15, Pages R565-R566 (August 2006)
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Marine Biology: New Light on Growth in the Cold
Brain Stimulation: Feeling the Buzz
Small RNAs: How Seeds Remember To Obey Their Mother
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Goal-Driven Behavioral Adaptations in Gap-Climbing Drosophila
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Pygmy mole crickets jump from water
Grasping Weber's law Current Biology
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
A Visual Sense of Number
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Basal bodies Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Volume 24, Issue 20, Pages R987-R988 (October 2014)
Presentation transcript:

Stepping of the forelegs over obstacles establishes long-lasting memories in cats  David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology  Volume 17, Issue 16, Pages R621-R623 (August 2007) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.026 Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Long-lasting memories are formed when cats step over obstacles with their forelegs. (A) Cats were stopped either after stepping over (left), or immediately before stepping over (right), an obstacle (height ∼7 cm). While distracted by a food dish, the obstacle was lowered into the walking surface (dotted line shows the initial obstacle position). (B) Maximum hind leg step height (measure at the toe) for different pause durations for two cats. When the cats straddled the obstacle (solid circles) the step remained high independent of pause duration, indicating a long-lasting memory of the obstacle. When the obstacle was not straddled (open circles), the step height became more variable and lower after short durations, indicating no consistent memory of the obstacle. Dotted lines in the two panels show the average hind leg step height during steps with no obstacle (standard deviations indicated by width of grey bars). Current Biology 2007 17, R621-R623DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.026) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Visual input is not necessary for the formation of long-lasting memories of obstacles. (A) While distracted by a food dish, a clear rod (∼7.5 cm above walking surface, shown by black dot) was placed in front of the cat's forelegs (left). By moving the food dish forward, the cats were encouraged to step forward, touching the rod with the foreleg, and eventually straddling it (right). The rod was subsequently removed. (B) Maximum hind leg step height of the leading step over the position of the rod for a range of pause durations for three cats. Note the maximum hind leg step height on most trials was well above the heights on control trials with no obstacle (dotted lines in the three panels, standard deviations indicated by width of grey bars). Current Biology 2007 17, R621-R623DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.026) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions