Mood as Representation of Momentum

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Luiz Pessoa Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 13, Issue 4, Pages (April 2009) DOI: /j.tics
Advertisements

Two views of brain function Marcus E. Raichle Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 14, Issue 4, Pages (April 2010) DOI: /j.tics
Elizabeth V. Goldfarb, Marvin M. Chun, Elizabeth A. Phelps  Neuron 
Volume 69, Issue 3, Pages (February 2011)
To Detect and Correct: Norm Violations and Their Enforcement
Cognitive neuroscience 2
Volume 80, Issue 4, Pages (November 2013)
Emotion, Decision Making, and the Amygdala
Rhythmic Working Memory Activation in the Human Hippocampus
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Volume 72, Issue 4, Pages (November 2011)
Volume 58, Issue 4, Pages (May 2008)
Frontal Cortex and the Discovery of Abstract Action Rules
Volume 76, Issue 5, Pages (December 2012)
Alan N. Hampton, Ralph Adolphs, J. Michael Tyszka, John P. O'Doherty 
Advances in fMRI Real-Time Neurofeedback
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e6 (April 2017)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Keno Juechems, Jan Balaguer, Maria Ruz, Christopher Summerfield  Neuron 
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Nils Kolling, Marco Wittmann, Matthew F.S. Rushworth  Neuron 
Perceptual Learning and Decision-Making in Human Medial Frontal Cortex
Volume 93, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 65, Issue 1, Pages (January 2010)
Hedging Your Bets by Learning Reward Correlations in the Human Brain
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Decoding Cognitive Processes from Neural Ensembles
The Psychology and Neuroscience of Financial Decision Making
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e6 (April 2017)
Visual Cortex Extrastriate Body-Selective Area Activation in Congenitally Blind People “Seeing” by Using Sounds  Ella Striem-Amit, Amir Amedi  Current.
Benedikt Zoefel, Alan Archer-Boyd, Matthew H. Davis  Current Biology 
Volume 74, Issue 3, Pages (May 2012)
Figure 1 We consider a scenario where the distributions of signals under safe or dangerous situations mean that a ... Figure 1 We consider a scenario where.
Between Thoughts and Actions: Motivationally Salient Cues Invigorate Mental Action in the Human Brain  Avi Mendelsohn, Alex Pine, Daniela Schiller  Neuron 
Optimal gene expression analysis by microarrays
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Rethinking Extinction
Joseph T. McGuire, Matthew R. Nassar, Joshua I. Gold, Joseph W. Kable 
Know Your Place: Neural Processing of Social Hierarchy in Humans
Processing of Social and Monetary Rewards in the Human Striatum
Kerstin Preuschoff, Peter Bossaerts, Steven R. Quartz  Neuron 
Fast Sequences of Non-spatial State Representations in Humans
Subliminal Instrumental Conditioning Demonstrated in the Human Brain
James M. Jeanne, Tatyana O. Sharpee, Timothy Q. Gentner  Neuron 
Marlene R. Cohen, John H.R. Maunsell  Neuron 
Volume 88, Issue 4, Pages (November 2015)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Neural Signatures of Economic Preferences for Risk and Ambiguity
Michael J. Frank, Brion S. Woroch, Tim Curran  Neuron 
Megan E. Speer, Jamil P. Bhanji, Mauricio R. Delgado  Neuron 
Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum  John P. O'Doherty, Tony W.
Arielle Tambini, Nicholas Ketz, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
How Dopamine Enhances an Optimism Bias in Humans
Volume 92, Issue 2, Pages (October 2016)
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
David Badre, Bradley B. Doll, Nicole M. Long, Michael J. Frank  Neuron 
Volume 62, Issue 4, Pages (May 2009)
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Volume 76, Issue 5, Pages (December 2012)
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
Fig. 1 Behavioral task and hypotheses.
Looking for cognition in the structure within the noise
Mental Imagery: Functional Mechanisms and Clinical Applications
Striatal Activity Underlies Novelty-Based Choice in Humans
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
Volume 28, Issue 18, Pages e3 (September 2018)
Keno Juechems, Jan Balaguer, Maria Ruz, Christopher Summerfield  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

Mood as Representation of Momentum Eran Eldar, Robb B. Rutledge, Raymond J. Dolan, Yael Niv  Trends in Cognitive Sciences  Volume 20, Issue 1, Pages 15-24 (January 2016) DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010 Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 The Effect of a Monetary Outcome on Mood and on Subsequent Neural and Behavioral Responses to Rewards. (A) Experimental design of [38]. To manipulate mood, a one-shot wheel-of-fortune (WoF) draw was held in between games, resulting in a gain or loss of $7. Game 1 and Game 2 involved different sets of slot machines with similar reward probabilities, and participants learned about the machines by trial and error. In the Test phase, participants chose between slot machines from Game 1 and Game 2. (B) In participants who reported high emotional instability, the WoF outcome affected self-reported mood (left, n=28 per group; 1 is maximally happy and −1 is maximally unhappy) and striatal BOLD response to reward measured by fMRI (middle, n=13 per group) during Game 2 as compared to Game 1 (shown are t values). In the test phase, those participants who experienced a WoF win preferred Game 2 machines, which they had played while in a better mood. By contrast, participants who had experienced a WoF loss preferred Game 1 machines, which they played before the WoF draw (right, n=28 per group; 1 indicates complete preference for Game 2 machines). * P < 0.05, ** P < 0.001. Adapted from [38]. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2016 20, 15-24DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Schematic of Possible Mood Dysfunctions. (A) (Top) Given a similar rate of learning in response to positive and negative outcomes, an environment in which positive and negative outcomes are equally likely leads to neutral expectations and a neutral mood on average. (Bottom) A lower rate of learning from negative outcomes leads to optimistic expectations and therefore larger negative prediction errors and persistent negative mood, a symptom seen in major depressive disorder. (B) Escalatory positive-feedback dynamics might turn mood into a ‘self-fulfilling prophecy’, leading to emotional instability, a major symptom of bipolar disorder. Positive surprises improve mood, biasing perception of outcomes upwards, thereby increasing the frequency and magnitude of further positive surprises. Optimistic expectations develop owing to the biased perception of outcomes. Mood stabilizes once expectations catch up with perceived outcomes, but subsequent outcomes, whose perception in now unbiased, then tend to fall short of optimistic expectations. Thus, negative surprises follow, thereby diminishing mood and biasing perception of outcomes downward. Similar positive-feedback dynamics then engender pessimistic expectations, setting the stage for the cycle to repeat, oscillating between good and bad mood indefinitely even if there are no changes in the actual distribution of outcomes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2016 20, 15-24DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure I Subjective Well-Being, Model Predictions, and Neural Activity. (A) Happiness ratings during a probabilistic reward task for two example participants. The model predicts ratings based on the past history of expectations and reward prediction errors (RPEs) resulting from those expectations. (B) BOLD activity in the ventral striatum measured by fMRI was correlated with subsequent happiness ratings, consistent with striatal representation of RPEs contributing to changes in mood. Adapted from [26]. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2016 20, 15-24DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure I Probabilistic Kalman–Filter [80] Models of the Environment. The reward outcome r at time-step t is sampled from a normal distribution whose mean vts is specific to the current state. (A) For a particular state, depicted here, changes in the mean follow a random walk with normally distributed noise. (B) A general environmental factor affects multiple states. At each time-step t, a general factor gt is sampled from a normal distribution whose mean is zero, and is then added to multiple state means (vts). (C) Changes in reward follow an underlying momentum. The mean reward vts of a state is sampled from a normal distribution whose mean is the sum of the previous mean vt−1s and the current momentum mt. Changes in momentum follow a random walk. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2016 20, 15-24DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions