Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Blur and Disparity Are Complementary Cues to Depth
Advertisements

Calibrating color vision
Volume 23, Issue 19, Pages (October 2013)
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Musical Consonance: The Importance of Harmonicity
What Image Properties Regulate Eye Growth?
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
A new galloping gait in an insect
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals
Thigmomorphogenesis Current Biology
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
There's more to magic than meets the eye
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R450-R452 (June 2017)
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Blur and Disparity Are Complementary Cues to Depth
Vision: When Does Looking Bigger Mean Seeing Better?
The many colours of ‘the dress’
Volume 23, Issue 9, Pages R364-R365 (May 2013)
Visual motion interferes with lexical decision on motion words
Myopia: The Importance of Seeing Fine Detail
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
At Birth, Humans Associate “Few” with Left and “Many” with Right
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Volume 45, Issue 5, Pages (March 2005)
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Volume 21, Issue 18, Pages R678-R679 (September 2011)
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Optic flow induces spatial filtering in fruit flies
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Visual Neuroscience: A Retinal Ganglion Cell to Report Image Focus?
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages R188-R189 (March 2015)
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Stephen G. Lomber, Blake E. Butler  Current Biology 
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Attention Reorients Periodically
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Neuroanatomy: Connectome Connects Fly and Mammalian Brain Networks
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 
A Visual Sense of Number
Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1
Notch Signaling: Filopodia Dynamics Confer Robustness
Basal bodies Current Biology
Thigmomorphogenesis Current Biology
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Presentation transcript:

Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited Frank H. Durgin  Current Biology  Volume 18, Issue 18, Pages R855-R856 (September 2008) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.053 Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Examples of textures previously used to study transfer across texture types [4]. The textures differed in spatial frequency content (above) and both orientation and spatial frequency (below). Aftereffects were strongest when the test texture was of the same type as the adaptation texture [4]. The elements at the top are more completely segregated in their power spectra than the textures Burr and Ross [1] used for their size manipulation. Huk and I [4] found greater transfer of effects from lower spatial-frequency textures to higher ones than vice-versa. Current Biology 2008 18, R855-R856DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.053) Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Effects of area manipulation. When the adapting number is greater on the left (400 versus 290) and the adapting density greater on the right (top texture pair, 11 versus 32 dots degree−2), the perceived numerosity/density of small test fields (lower pair) responds to the difference in adapting density, not number. Three observers naïve to the hypotheses were adapted and tested (elements were white on black, with the small test patches approximately 3 degrees in diameter). To remove effects of local elements, four successive images of statistically equivalent adapting textures preceded each test trial. Blank intervals were added between images to remove effects of apparent motion. Following adaptation, an average of 61 ± 7 (SD) dots presented on the left was adequate to appear equal to 100 dots presented on the right. That is, a texture in the region adapted to the denser side appeared less dense (and therefore less numerous) than dots presented within the region adapted to the more numerous, but less dense texture. The figure may be used to experience the effect by fixating the upper x for about 30 seconds and then glancing to the lower one while comparing the relative densities of the two small texture patches (which are the same patch rotated by 180 degrees). This experiment indicates that density is the adapting dimension. Current Biology 2008 18, R855-R856DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.053) Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions