A simple rule of thumb for elegant prehension

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Correlation/Regression - part 2 Consider Example 2.12 in section 2.3. Look at the scatterplot… Example 2.13 shows that the prediction line is given by.
Advertisements

Heterogeneous Coding of Temporally Discounted Values in the Dorsal and Ventral Striatum during Intertemporal Choice  Xinying Cai, Soyoun Kim, Daeyeol.
Evidence for Surface-Based Processing of Binocular Disparity
The Dynamic Range of Human Lightness Perception
Animal Cognition: How Archer Fish Learn to Down Rapidly Moving Targets
Use of Visual Force Feedback to Improve Digit Force Direction During Pinch Grip in Persons With Stroke: A Pilot Study  Na Jin Seo, PhD, Heidi W. Fischer,
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Volume 15, Issue 7, Pages (April 2005)
Bipedal Locomotion in Crawling Cells
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Sergei Gepshtein, Martin S. Banks  Current Biology 
Ingvars Birznieks, Richard M. Vickery  Current Biology 
Beauty Requires Thought
Estimation of changes in cardiac output from the arterial blood pressure waveform in the upper limb†,††  N.W.F. Linton, R.A.F. Linton  British Journal.
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Arne Gennerich, Detlev Schild  Biophysical Journal 
Motion-Based Analysis of Spatial Patterns by the Human Visual System
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
Volume 111, Issue 2, Pages (July 2016)
Cascaded Effects of Spatial Adaptation in the Early Visual System
Margarita V. Chibalina, Dmitry A. Filatov  Current Biology 
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Spatial Coding of the Predicted Impact Location of a Looming Object
Cortical Mechanisms of Smooth Eye Movements Revealed by Dynamic Covariations of Neural and Behavioral Responses  David Schoppik, Katherine I. Nagel, Stephen.
Volume 24, Issue 13, Pages (July 2014)
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Volume 28, Issue 5, Pages e3 (March 2018)
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Singular Behavior of Slow Dynamics of Single Excitable Cells
Directional Selectivity Is Formed at Multiple Levels by Laterally Offset Inhibition in the Rabbit Retina  Shelley I. Fried, Thomas A. Mu¨nch, Frank S.
Sensory Population Decoding for Visually Guided Movements
Volume 14, Issue 7, Pages (February 2016)
Volume 90, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Martin Eimer, Anna Grubert  Current Biology 
Spatial Coding of the Predicted Impact Location of a Looming Object
Brian D Corneil, Etienne Olivier, Douglas P Munoz  Neuron 
Jeremy B. Wilmer, Ken Nakayama  Neuron 
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Andrew D. Howard, MD, Robin S. Howard, MA, Stuart L
Greg Schwartz, Sam Taylor, Clark Fisher, Rob Harris, Michael J. Berry 
Mathematical Models of Protein Kinase Signal Transduction
Volume 105, Issue 1, Pages (July 2013)
Timescales of Inference in Visual Adaptation
A Large-Conductance Anion Channel of the Golgi Complex
Artem G. Ayuyan, Fredric S. Cohen  Biophysical Journal 
Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex
Computer Use Changes Generalization of Movement Learning
Acyl Chain Length and Saturation Modulate Interleaflet Coupling in Asymmetric Bilayers: Effects on Dynamics and Structural Order  Salvatore Chiantia,
Rheological Analysis and Measurement of Neutrophil Indentation
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Angela M. Haffenden, Karen C. Schiff, Melvyn A. Goodale 
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
When Correlation Implies Causation in Multisensory Integration
Top-Down Modulation of Lateral Interactions in Early Vision
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Proprioceptive Distance Cues Restore Perfect Size Constancy in Grasping, but Not Perception, When Vision Is Limited  Juan Chen, Irene Sperandio, Melvyn.
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Reliability of Assessment of Protein Structure Prediction Methods
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Alexandra B Nelson, Claudia M Krispel, Chris Sekirnjak, Sascha du Lac 
Experimental Verification of the Behavioral Foundation of Bacterial Transport Parameters Using Microfluidics  Tanvir Ahmed, Roman Stocker  Biophysical.
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages (November 2006)
George D. Dickinson, Ian Parker  Biophysical Journal 
Presentation transcript:

A simple rule of thumb for elegant prehension Mark Mon-Williams, James R. Tresilian  Current Biology  Volume 11, Issue 13, Pages 1058-1061 (July 2001) DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00293-7

Figure 1 (a) A schematic of reach-to-grasp movement. The horizontal dashed line indicates the transport of the thumb to the target (filled box), while the vertical dashed line indicates the opening of the grasp component. A “typical” (i.e., observed commonly in standard prehension experiments) wrist velocity (solid line) and aperture (dashed line) profile is shown with the low-velocity portion of the transport movement identified. The digits open to a maximum aperture (do) before closing in on the object (covering a distance dc). (b) The relative time to the point of maximum grip aperture (MGA), determined from the ratio between do and dc, expressed as the percentage of total movement duration, and plotted against object width (W). The solid line plots the relationship obtained when the median empirically observed relationship between MGA and object width [2] is used to calculate do/dc. The lower and upper lines show the relationship when the bias is increased and decreased, respectively. Two sets of data from an empirical study are shown (see Figure 6b in [2] for a plot of the data from 32 studies). The filled circles relate to a condition in which visual feedback was present during prehension, while the unfilled circles relate to a condition in which there was no visual feedback. It can be seen that MGA occurred relatively later, as object size increased in both conditions. In the condition without visual feedback, MGA was wider (the relationship between MGA and object size had a gain of 0.7 and a bias of 3.8 cm) and occurred relatively earlier (with visual feedback, the relationship between MGA and object size had a gain of 0.8 and a bias of 2.5 cm) Current Biology 2001 11, 1058-1061DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00293-7)

Figure 2 (a) A schematic of the targets used in the experiment. (b) The actual time (ms) of maximum grip aperture plotted against the predicted time (ms). The different symbols indicate the different conditions (different starting separation between finger and thumb). The line shows the least-square quadratic regression across all conditions. It can be seen that 96% of the timing variance is accounted for by the simple rule of thumb Current Biology 2001 11, 1058-1061DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00293-7)