Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The longer one looks, the less one sees --- catching the features
Advertisements

Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
Visual Influences on Echo Suppression
Joshua J. Foster, Emma M. Bsales, Russell J. Jaffe, Edward Awh 
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Volume 27, Issue 7, Pages (April 2017)
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Volume 26, Issue 14, Pages (July 2016)
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Bottom-Up Versus Top-Down
Depth Affects Where We Look
Perirhinal-Hippocampal Connectivity during Reactivation Is a Marker for Object-Based Memory Consolidation  Kaia L. Vilberg, Lila Davachi  Neuron  Volume.
Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing
Ayelet McKyton, Itay Ben-Zion, Ravid Doron, Ehud Zohary 
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e2 (October 2017)
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Volume 18, Issue 24, Pages (December 2008)
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Mathilde Bonnefond, Ole Jensen  Current Biology 
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Martin Eimer, Anna Grubert  Current Biology 
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages (September 2012)
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Kyoko Yoshida, Nobuhito Saito, Atsushi Iriki, Masaki Isoda 
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Near-Real-Time Feature-Selective Modulations in Human Cortex
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Kristy A. Sundberg, Jude F. Mitchell, John H. Reynolds  Neuron 
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Hippocampal-Prefrontal Theta Oscillations Support Memory Integration
Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, Stefano Panzeri  Current Biology 
Color Constancy for an Unseen Surface
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Presentation transcript:

Interference with Bottom-Up Feature Detection by Higher-Level Object Recognition  Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology  Volume 17, Issue 1, Pages 26-31 (January 2007) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.050 Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Small Portions of Visual-Search Displays The target possesses the uniquely left- or right-tilted (as in these examples) bar in the entire display. In conditions Asimple and Bsimple (top), all bars were 45° from vertical, except the target bar in Bsimple, which was 20° from vertical (in this example) or horizontal. Conditions A and B (bottom), derived from Asimple and Bsimple, differed only in the angle, 45° and 20°, respectively, between the two bars in the target. Task-irrelevant, horizontal and vertical, bars made the orientation singleton much harder to find in condition A than in condition B. Current Biology 2007 17, 26-31DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.050) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Hand and Gaze Responses in Experiment I (A and B) Examples of gaze scan paths. The one in (A) is for an arrive-abandon-return (AAR) trial. Asterisks and open circles mark the locations for targets and fixation points, respectively; the grid frames the spatial extent of the stimuli. Blue and red scan paths, respectively, are for those before and after the first gaze's arrival to the target and before the button press. (C) Data for three subjects, denoted by red, green, and blue hues, respectively. Asterisks indicate significant differences between conditions for the subject. The left graphs show RThand and RTeye (top of lighter- and darker-colored bars, respectively) for button responses and first gaze at target, respectively. The right graphs show task performances, percentage of arrive-abandon return (AAR) scan paths (e.g., [A]), and eye-to-hand latencies in non-AAR trials for conditions A and B only. All error bars show SEM. Current Biology 2007 17, 26-31DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.050) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Experiment II: The Longer One Looks, the Worse One “Sees” (A) Sequence of events in a gaze-contingent trial. (B) A small portion of an example of a mask stimulus. (C) With longer gaze-to-mask latency T, target localization in condition A (in blocked sessions) worsened, and the gazes are more likely to have abandoned the target before mask onset. Asterisks denote data points significantly smaller in value than that for T = 0. GSBM trials are those in which gaze stayed (at target) before mask onset. (D) In sessions interleaving conditions A and B (for another subject group), performances in conditions A and B are comparable for T = 0. Combining both T > 0 values, performance in B is significantly better than that in A (p = 0.01). Error bars show SEM. Current Biology 2007 17, 26-31DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.050) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Factors Affecting Object-to-Feature Interference (A) In experiment II, performance across T > 0 for condition A was somewhat better (p = 0.08) in blocked sessions (one session each subject) than in sessions interleaved with condition B (two sessions each subject), in which subjects had higher expectations for uniquely shaped target. (B) Reduction of interference with experience—for longer gaze-to-mask time T in experiment II, performance for condition A improved in the second experimental session (significantly at T value with an asterisk next to the data points). The data in Figure 3D were replotted here according to the two separate sessions. (C) Stronger or weaker object-to-feature interference, manifested in RThand − RTeye in experiment I, by, respectively, higher or lower orientation variabilities of the distractors for reducing or enhancing bottom-up pop-out strength manifested in RTeye (same three subjects as those in Figure 2, denoted by blue, red, and green colors). The RThand − RTeye in condition A′, although reduced from that of A, is significantly longer (p = 0.002) than that of B′. In the right graph, data points of different conditions are plotted in different colors. Stimulus examples of conditions A′, B′, and A′simple are shown in the Supplemental Data. Error bars show SEM. Current Biology 2007 17, 26-31DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.050) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions