Nori Jacoby, Josh H. McDermott  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Advertisements

Joshua J. Foster, Emma M. Bsales, Russell J. Jaffe, Edward Awh 
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Volume 25, Issue 21, Pages (November 2015)
Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Norm-Based Coding of Voice Identity in Human Auditory Cortex
Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids
Lexical Influences on Auditory Streaming
Huan Luo, Xing Tian, Kun Song, Ke Zhou, David Poeppel  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Depth Affects Where We Look
Nori Jacoby, Josh H. McDermott  Current Biology 
Selective Attention in an Insect Visual Neuron
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Aryeh Hai Taub, Rita Perets, Eilat Kahana, Rony Paz  Neuron 
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Visual Cortex Extrastriate Body-Selective Area Activation in Congenitally Blind People “Seeing” by Using Sounds  Ella Striem-Amit, Amir Amedi  Current.
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Volume 27, Issue 16, Pages e6 (August 2017)
Benedikt Zoefel, Alan Archer-Boyd, Matthew H. Davis  Current Biology 
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Serial Dependence in the Perception of Faces
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e3 (December 2017)
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages (September 2012)
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Timing, Timing, Timing: Fast Decoding of Object Information from Intracranial Field Potentials in Human Visual Cortex  Hesheng Liu, Yigal Agam, Joseph.
Elena A. Allen, Erik B. Erhardt, Vince D. Calhoun  Neuron 
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Attention Reorients Periodically
Humans Have an Expectation That Gaze Is Directed Toward Them
Raghav Rajan, Allison J. Doupe  Current Biology 
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
When Correlation Implies Causation in Multisensory Integration
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Rapid Formation of Robust Auditory Memories: Insights from Noise
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
Hippocampal-Prefrontal Theta Oscillations Support Memory Integration
Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, Stefano Panzeri  Current Biology 
Color Constancy for an Unseen Surface
Coupled Oscillator Dynamics of Vocal Turn-Taking in Monkeys
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
A Visual Sense of Number
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in Olfaction
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Integer Ratio Priors on Musical Rhythm Revealed Cross-culturally by Iterated Reproduction  Nori Jacoby, Josh H. McDermott  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages 359-370 (February 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Illustration of Stimuli and Experimental Paradigm (A) One repetition of a three-interval rhythm (four clicks) with integer ratio proportions (1:1:2), resulting in a rhythm with intervals of 25%, 25%, and 50% of the total pattern duration. (B) One repetition of a three-interval rhythm with non-integer ratio proportions (1.6:2.14:1). (C) Three repetitions of the 1:1:2 three-interval rhythm (ten clicks). (D) The repeated pattern 1:1:2 written in Western musical notation. (E) As first described in [20], three-interval rhythms can be presented as a point in a three-dimensional space, where the x, y, and z coordinates correspond to the first, second, and third interval, respectively. The points with an overall fixed duration (2,000 ms in this case) form a triangle (simplex) that can be projected to a two-dimensional plane, creating a “rhythm simplex,” also known as a “chronotopological map,” or “rhythm chart” [20, 27]). Here and in (F), the dashed triangle denotes the region from which stimuli were drawn (defined by inter-onset intervals that are larger than 15% of the total duration, e.g., 300 ms for the 2,000 ms duration used in most experiments), with two example integer ratio rhythms indicated by crosses. (F) The rhythm simplex. The red line plots an example stimulus/response trajectory across the five iterations of an example trial. (G) Schematic of trial structure. Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Results of Experiment 1: Iterated Reproduction by Synchronous Tapping (A) All seeds of experiment 1 displayed on the rhythm simplex. (B) All responses from each of the five iterations of experiment 1. (C) Kernel density estimate of the continuous distribution underlying the data from iteration 5 of experiment 1. Crosses plot simple integer ratio rhythms. See also Figures S1, S2, and S4. Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Results of Experiment 2: Tapping versus Vocalizing (A) Schematic of the tapping task. (B) Schematic of the vocalization task. (C) Response distribution from the fifth iteration of the tapping task. (D) Response distribution from the fifth iteration of the vocalization task. (E) Weights of Gaussian mixture components assigned to each integer ratio rhythm in the two distributions (see also Figure S7). Error bars plot confidence intervals on the weights (SDs of the weight distributions derived from bootstrapping), with significance here and elsewhere evaluated after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Results of Experiment 3: Synchronization and Replication (A) Schematic of the synchronization task. (B) Schematic of the replication task. (C) Response distribution from the fifth iteration of the synchronization task. (D) Response distribution from the fifth iteration of the replication task. (E) Weights of Gaussian mixture components assigned to each integer ratio rhythm in the two distributions. Error bars plot confidence intervals on the weights (SDs of the weight distributions derived from bootstrapping). See also Figure S3. Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Results of Experiment 5: Rhythm Discrimination, with Predictions from the Measured Prior (A) The response distribution from experiment S2 (with a pattern duration of 1,000 ms). (B) Close-up view of a slice through rhythm space occupied by experimental stimuli. Stimuli are depicted with white crosses, and perceptual biases predicted by the prior are marked as magenta diamonds; yellow arrows show direction of bias. Stimuli are “pulled” toward the modes, such that estimates of points straddling the trough are further apart than those of points on one side of the trough, improving discrimination. (C) Hit and correct rejection rates for human listeners and for an ideal observer using the measured prior. Error bars plot the SEM. (D) Perceptual sensitivity for each position along the continuum, along with the predicted sensitivity of the ideal observer from (C). The value plotted for position k is the discriminability of the rhythms at positions k−1 and k+1. Values are thus available only for positions 2 through 6. Error bars plot the SEM. See also Figure S7. Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 6 Results of Experiment 6: Speaking versus Tapping (A) Schematic of the speaking task. Listeners heard a spoken phrase, segmented into syllables that were assigned random onset times, and repeated it. (B) Schematic of the tapping task. (C) Spectrogram of an example speaking stimulus. Silences of variable durations were inserted between syllables such that the intervals between voicing onsets (dashed red lines) had the desired intervals—in this example, r1=1000, r2=500, and r3=500 ms (for illustrative purposes). (D) Spectrogram of example recorded response. Dotted horizontal lines mark the two ranges used for the onset detection process (range I, white: 40–500 Hz; range II, red: 2700–6000 Hz). Vertical dashed lines mark the detected onsets. (E) Response distribution from the fifth iteration of the speaking task, for each of five phrases. (F) Response distribution from the fifth iteration of the tapping task. See also Figures S5 and S6. Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 7 Priors in Amazonians and US Participants with and without Musical Training (A–C) Response distribution from fifth iteration of tapping task for Tsimané participants (A), US non-musicians (B), and US musicians (C). (D and E) Weights of Gaussian mixture components assigned to each integer ratio rhythm for Tsimané and age-matched US participants with little musical exposure (D) and US musicians and non-musicians (E). Error bars plot confidence intervals on the weights (SDs of the weight distributions derived from bootstrapping). Current Biology 2017 27, 359-370DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.031) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions