New Insights in Human Memory Interference and Consolidation

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Memory systems Off-line processing, consolidation, and interference.
Advertisements

Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Emotional Memory: Selective Enhancement by Sleep
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R447-R448 (June 2017)
Eukaryotic Evolution: The Importance of Being Archaebacterial
Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning
Jean-Paul Noel, Mark Wallace, Randolph Blake  Current Biology 
Transcriptional Memory: Staying in the Loop
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Obesity: Cognitive Impairment and the Failure to ‘Eat Right’
Spatial Memory: Mice Quickly Learn a Safe Haven
Human Memory: Brain-State-Dependent Effects of Stimulation
Episodic Memory: Rats Master Multiple Memories
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Gene Evolution: Getting Something from Nothing
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Comparative Cognition: Action Imitation Using Episodic Memory
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Transcranial magnetic stimulation
Cell Division: Experiments and Modelling Unite to Resolve the Middle
Tzvi Ganel, Eran Chajut, Daniel Algom  Current Biology 
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Infant cognition Current Biology
Neural Coding: Time Contraction and Dilation in the Striatum
X-Inactivation: Xist RNA Uses Chromosome Contacts to Coat the X
Memory Processing: Ripples in the Resting Brain
Volume 17, Issue 21, Pages (November 2007)
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Hedgehog Signaling: Is Smo a G Protein-Coupled Receptor?
Gene Regulation: The HSP70 Gene Jumps When Shocked
Modification of Existing Human Motor Memories Is Enabled by Primary Cortical Processing during Memory Reactivation  Nitzan Censor, Michael A. Dimyan,
American birds: Audubon was not the first
Visual Cortex: The Eccentric Area Prostriata in the Human Brain
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 21, Issue 11, Pages R414-R415 (June 2011)
Technology for Chronic Pain
Neuroscience: The Rhythms of Speech Understanding
Meiosis: SYP Up Firmly and Cross Over Evenly
Quantity Cognition: Numbers, Numerosity, Zero and Mathematics
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Meiosis: SYP Up Firmly and Cross Over Evenly
Purple Tomatoes: Longer Lasting, Less Disease, and Better for You
Better Fruits and Vegetables through Sensory Analysis
Interplay of Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex in Memory
Brain Imaging: Decoding Your Memories
Gene Evolution: Getting Something from Nothing
Volume 18, Issue 12, Pages R527-R530 (June 2008)
Sense of agency Current Biology
Volume 20, Issue 9, Pages (May 2010)
Cryptosporidium Current Biology
It’s all about the constraints
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Learning: Plasticity without Stabilization in Olfactory Cortex
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Anemonefishes Current Biology
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
American birds: Audubon was not the first
Transcranial magnetic stimulation
Basal bodies Current Biology
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Vision: Attending the Invisible
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Presentation transcript:

New Insights in Human Memory Interference and Consolidation Edwin M. Robertson  Current Biology  Volume 22, Issue 2, Pages R66-R71 (January 2012) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.051 Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Memory interference. (A) Immediately following its acquisition or retrieval a memory (M) is susceptible to interference, and so a disruptive technique such as applying TMS (coil) over a particular brain area can impair recall. (B) By contrast, after acquisition a memory is consolidated, and similarly following its retrieval a memory is reconsolidated, making the memory resistant to interference. So, applying TMS several hours (for example, +6 hours) after acquisition or retrieval has little or no effect upon recall because by this time the memory has been stabilized and is no longer susceptible to interference. Current Biology 2012 22, R66-R71DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.051) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Examples of memory organization in the human brain. (A) There may be independent procedural (P; for example, a learning motor skill) and declarative (D; for example, learning a word-list) memory systems within the human brain. For example, impairment in declarative learning can occur without any impairment in procedural learning [9]. (B) Alternatively, there may be an interaction between different types of memories. Functional imaging work has shown that there is a correlation between activity within the procedural (the striatum) and declarative memory systems (the MTL) [13]. So, there is a functional coupling between memory systems with activity within one memory system affecting the other, and this coupling could support interactions between different memories. (C) There may only be a partial overlap between declarative and procedural processing, which would explain word-list learning impairing only a component, but not all of a motor skill [16]. (D) Brain areas can control the interference between memories. When the brain state is altered artificially (blur) — for example, by applying TMS — there is no longer interference of a motor skill by a word-list memory (blurred line) [22]. The organization of human memories may also shift naturally depending upon brain state; for example, interference occurs between memories during wakefulness but not during sleep [15,17,21]. Current Biology 2012 22, R66-R71DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.051) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Memory interference and the prefrontal cortex. (A) Participants learnt a spatial location task while being exposed to a rose petal odor. The odor was subsequently used to reactivate the spatial memory when participants were either awake or asleep. Immediately after reactivation participants performed an interference task. (B) There was a decrease in memory recall and activation of the DLPFC in the wake group, whereas there was an increase in recall and little activation of the DLPFC in the sleep group. Thus, for a memory to be susceptible to interference — as shown by a decrease in memory recall — the DLPFC has to be activated. (C) In another set of experiments, participants learnt a word-list (red box; Recall1) and a motor skill (blue box) in quick succession, sham TMS or real TMS was applied to the DLPFC, and participants recall for the word-list was retested (Recall2). (D) Interference from the motor skill task caused a substantial decrease in word-recall (Recall2 – Recall1) in the sham group. In contrast, this expected decrease in word-recall did not occur following DLPFC stimulation. (E) The correlation between memory interference (Δ Word recall) and initial motor skill, which was present following sham stimulation, was abolished by applying real stimulation to the DLPFC. Thus, disrupting the function of DLPFC by applying stimulation prevented interference between the tasks. Current Biology 2012 22, R66-R71DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.051) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions