Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Piercing of Consciousness as a Threshold-Crossing Operation
Advertisements

Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Volume 54, Issue 6, Pages (June 2007)
Do Motoneurons Encode the Noncommutativity of Ocular Rotations?
Animal Cognition: How Archer Fish Learn to Down Rapidly Moving Targets
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
Cursive Writing with Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Decoding Wakefulness Levels from Typical fMRI Resting-State Data Reveals Reliable Drifts between Wakefulness and Sleep  Enzo Tagliazucchi, Helmut Laufs 
Efficient Receptive Field Tiling in Primate V1
Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Joshua P. Bassett, Thomas J. Wills, Francesca Cacucci  Current Biology 
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Representation of Object Weight in Human Ventral Visual Cortex
Sergei Gepshtein, Martin S. Banks  Current Biology 
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
Face Pareidolia in the Rhesus Monkey
Relationship of Correlated Spontaneous Activity to Functional Ocular Dominance Columns in the Developing Visual Cortex  Chiayu Chiu, Michael Weliky  Neuron 
A Map for Horizontal Disparity in Monkey V2
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Do Motoneurons Encode the Noncommutativity of Ocular Rotations?
Volume 24, Issue 13, Pages (July 2014)
Jianing Yu, David Ferster  Neuron 
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Sleep Enhances Plasticity in the Developing Visual Cortex
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Guifen Chen, Daniel Manson, Francesca Cacucci, Thomas Joseph Wills 
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages (September 2012)
Jeremy B. Wilmer, Ken Nakayama  Neuron 
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Medial Axis Shape Coding in Macaque Inferotemporal Cortex
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Guilhem Ibos, David J. Freedman  Neuron 
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Optic flow induces spatial filtering in fruit flies
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Robust Selectivity to Two-Object Images in Human Visual Cortex
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Noa Raz, Ella Striem, Golan Pundak, Tanya Orlov, Ehud Zohary 
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 24, Issue 8, Pages e6 (August 2018)
Albert V. van den Berg, Jaap A. Beintema  Neuron 
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Visual Perception: A Novel Difference Channel in Binocular Vision
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Volume 29, Issue 5, Pages e4 (March 2019)
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception
Motion Adaptation and the Velocity Coding of Natural Scenes
Efficient Receptive Field Tiling in Primate V1
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in Olfaction
Presentation transcript:

Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale Martina Poletti, Murat Aytekin, Michele Rucci  Current Biology  Volume 25, Issue 24, Pages 3253-3259 (December 2015) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.004 Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Ocular Drift Characteristics The eyes move incessantly during visual fixation. (Top) Examples of ocular drift traces in the two eyes are shown. (Bottom) Probability distributions of ocular drift velocities are shown. The two panels show data from the right and left eye for one observer. Marginal distributions for the vertical and horizontal velocity components are shown on the corresponding axes. Numbers represent means and SDs of the instantaneous speed. (A) Eye movements measured under strict head immobilization, a common condition in oculomotor experiments. The subject maintained fixation on a small marker while head movements were prevented by means of a bite bar and headrest. (B) Eye movements (rotations of the eyes within the head) were acquired during normal head-free fixation (see also the Supplemental Experimental Procedures and Figure S1). Current Biology 2015 25, 3253-3259DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.004) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Binocular Drift Coordination Joint probability distributions for the instantaneous velocities of ocular drift in the two eyes. Colored lines represent iso-probability contours. The two columns refer to different conditions; the two rows show horizontal and vertical velocity components. (A) Natural head-free binocular viewing is shown. (B) Head-fixed binocular viewing. In this condition, the subject’s head was firmly immobilized by a bite bar and a headrest. In all panels, the solid and dashed lines represent the linear regression and the 45° diagonal, respectively. Average correlation and slope of the linear regression are reported in each panel. Data represent averages across four observers. See also Figure S2 for the results of similar analysis under monocular viewing. Current Biology 2015 25, 3253-3259DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.004) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Fixational Head-Eye Coordination (A) An example of head and eye traces during steady fixation. Note the very small scale of the movements on the y axis. (B) Comparison between the velocities of head and eye rotations. The horizontal and vertical components of ocular drift velocity are plotted as a function of the angular speeds of head yaw and pitch, respectively. Solid and dashed lines represent the linear regression and the 45° diagonal, respectively. (C) Head-eye compensation gain. Distributions of the instantaneous gain between fixational head and eye movements are shown. On each axis, gain equal to one corresponds to perfect compensation (the fixated target remains immobile on the retina). Larger and smaller gain values represent over- and under-compensation, respectively. (D) Head-eye compensation delay. Mean cross-correlation functions between head and eye rotations. Shaded areas represent one SD. Data are averages across all the recorded eyes during normal binocular viewing. See also Figure S2 for the results of similar analysis under monocular viewing. Current Biology 2015 25, 3253-3259DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.004) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Retinal Image Motion (A) Examples of probability distributions of the velocity of the fixated target on the retina. (Top) Normal head-free fixation is shown. (Bottom) No head-eye coordination is shown. Compensation was disrupted by decoupling head and eye traces. (B) Average speeds of the retinal projection of the fixated target for the two conditions in (A). For comparison, the dashed line shows the average retinal speed measured in standard experiments with head immobilization, when subjects freely observed natural scenes. Error bars and the shaded region represent one SD. The asterisk marks statistical significance at p < 0.005 (two-tailed paired t test). (C) Retinal speed distributions in the sequential looking task under head-free and head-fixed conditions. Error bars represent SEM. (D) Binocular correlation in retinal image motion. Joint probability distributions of the velocity components in the projections of the fixated target in the two retinas are shown. Different rows compare horizontal and vertical velocities (arcmin/s). In (B)–(D), data represent averages across all recorded eyes. Current Biology 2015 25, 3253-3259DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.004) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions