A way of selectively degrading colour constancy demonstrates the experience dependence of colour vision  Eli Brenner, Frans W. Cornelissen  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Erman Misirlisoy, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
Advertisements

Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R447-R448 (June 2017)
Volume 24, Issue 23, Pages R1109-R1111 (December 2014)
Jean-Paul Noel, Mark Wallace, Randolph Blake  Current Biology 
The Dynamic Range of Human Lightness Perception
Cell Adhesion: Sizing Up a Sticky Situation
Plant Growth: Jogging the Cell Cycle with JAG
Vision and the light environment
Cryptic genetic variation
Colour Vision: Understanding #TheDress
Nuclear envelope Current Biology
Synaptic Physiology: Illuminating the Road Ahead
Green-Red Flashers to Accelerate Biology
Shape Control: Cell Growth Hits the Mechanical Buffers
Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Global primary production
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Nocturnal bees learn landmark colours in starlight
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Vision: Attention Makes the Cup Flow Over
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
A new galloping gait in an insect
Meiosis: Organizing Microtubule Organizers
Spatiochromatic Properties of Natural Images and Human Vision
G. Lorimer Moseley, Timothy J. Parsons, Charles Spence  Current Biology 
Emergent acoustic order in arrays of mosquitoes
Visual Perception: Lightness in a High-Dynamic-Range World
Sleep: How Many Switches Does It Take To Turn Off the Lights?
Shape Control: Cell Growth Hits the Mechanical Buffers
Volume 25, Issue 23, Pages R1114-R1116 (December 2015)
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Elephant cognition Current Biology
Volume 23, Issue 9, Pages R364-R365 (May 2013)
Publication metrics and success on the academic job market
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Elementary motion detectors
Volume 21, Issue 18, Pages R678-R679 (September 2011)
Fish choose appropriately when and with whom to collaborate
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
It’s all about the constraints
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Martijn Barendregt, Ben M. Harvey, Bas Rokers, Serge O. Dumoulin 
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Vision and lack of vision in the ocean
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Volume 22, Issue 8, Pages R264-R265 (April 2012)
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Neuronal Plasticity: How Do Neurons Know What To Do?
The Perception and Misperception of Specular Surface Reflectance
Visual Circuits: Division of Labor Revealed
Color Constancy for an Unseen Surface
Grasping Weber's law Current Biology
Attention-Dependent Representation of a Size Illusion in Human V1
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Vision and the light environment
Basal bodies Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Presentation transcript:

A way of selectively degrading colour constancy demonstrates the experience dependence of colour vision  Eli Brenner, Frans W. Cornelissen  Current Biology  Volume 15, Issue 21, Pages R864-R866 (November 2005) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.033 Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Judging surface colours despite changes in the illumination (colour constancy) and under alternating monochromatic illumination. We consider a scene with four surfaces that each reflect different amounts of the light that stimulates two kinds of cones within the eye. (A) For a given surface, the stimulation of each cone type (indicated schematically by the bars) depends on the spectral content of the light that the surface reflects (indicated by the colour of the slanted square), which is a product of the spectral content of the light source (lamp) and the fractions of different wavelengths of light that the surface reflects (indicated by portions of red and green disks). (B) Exactly the same cone stimulation (compare bars within dotted circles) can be obtained with a different combination of lamp and surface reflection. (C) One can distinguish, for instance, a yellow surface under a yellow lamp (A) from a reddish surface under a greenish lamp (B) by also considering the surrounding surfaces. Under the yellow lamp, as in (A), the neighbouring reddish surface, as in (B), will reflect more red and less green light. (D) Under the greenish lamp, as in (B), the neighbouring yellow surface, as in (A), will reflect less red and more green light. (E) For each cone type, the pattern of excitation across the four surfaces under the greenish illumination (right) is approximately a scaled version of the pattern under the yellow illumination (left). Scaling all the responses within each cone type by some factor, so that the average or peak value per cone has a fixed value, would therefore lead to colour constancy. (F) Under monochromatic illumination all surfaces appear to have the same colour (the ratio of the stimulation of the different kinds of cones is constant). Only the luminance — the overall level of cone stimulation — differs between the surfaces. When switching between different colours of monochromatic light, as in Sugita’s study [1], the luminance distribution changes, so that by comparing the relative luminance under the differently coloured lamps one could retrieve the colour of the surface. In that case it is a disadvantage to scale the responses as described above, because doing so makes it more difficult to judge the colour of the illumination. Current Biology 2005 15, R864-R866DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.033) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions