Hungarian preschoolers’ interpretation of doubly quantified sentences Katalin É. Kiss, Mátyás Gerőcs, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
A Bi-level Formulation for the Combined Dynamic Equilibrium based Traffic Signal Control Satish V. Ukkusuri, Associate Professor, Purdue University
Advertisements

Making the most of assessment: designing multi-purpose tasks for young language learners Louise Courtney Warsaw 21st October 2011.
5.1 Rules for Exponents Review of Bases and Exponents Zero Exponents
PDAs Accept Context-Free Languages
Properties Use, share, or modify this drill on mathematic properties. There is too much material for a single class, so you’ll have to select for your.
Method Participants 184 five-year-old (M age=5.63, SD=0.22) kindergarten students from 30 classrooms in central Illinois Teacher ratings The second edition.
The big picture A tough decade has come to an end and there now is an opportunity to strengthen the quality of education.
DRDP Measure Slides by Domain
Seeds for Early Literacy Oral Language California Preschool Instructional Network A project of the California Department of Education Child Development.
Presenter Name(s) Issue date National Student.
Leicestershire Numeracy Team 2003
1 Cognitive sociolinguistics Richard Hudson Budapest March 2012.
Oral Production and Error Correction Amongst Arab Learners of English
Tests of Hypotheses Based on a Single Sample
The Grade 9 Cohort of Fall 2000: Post-secondary Pathways Preliminary Analysis Presentation to HEQCO - June 15, 2009 Dr. Robert S. Brown Organizational.
REASONING WITH SUBJUNCTIVE (COUNTERFACTUAL) AND INDICATIVE CONDITIONALS A comparison of children, adolescents and adults Eva Rafetseder & Josef Perner.
What Works in Undergraduate Science Education? 1 Heidi Iverson, Colorado State University OECD France Workshop Education for Innovation: the Role of Arts.
EU Market Situation for Eggs and Poultry Management Committee 21 June 2012.
Replacement Skills Individualized Intensive Interventions:
Faculty of Arts University of Groningen The acquisition of the weak-strong distinction and the Dutch quantifier allemaal Erik-Jan Smits
1. Researcher: Jean-Remy Hochmann RA: Melody Wu 2.
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT, PART 1
Effects of Color versus Black and White Pictures on Children's Storytelling Buffy Dubreuil, Reane Laurin, and Phyllis Schneider Department of Speech Pathology.
How Primary School Children View University By Michael Lee (aged 11)
Functional Areas & Positions
Coursework (SL) 25% Coursework (HL) 20%.  The purpose of the internally assessed coursework is to amplify, reinforce and extend the principal geographical.
The Governments of the world decided that certain rules apply to ALL children under the age of 18 to help keep them safe and protect their needs.
1 Let’s Recapitulate. 2 Regular Languages DFAs NFAs Regular Expressions Regular Grammars.
The Third International Theraplay Conference 2007 Chicago Ulrike Franke and Herbert H.G. Wettig Presenter: Ulrike Franke, Cert. Theraplay Trainer - Supervisor.
Internet Filtering Requirements: CIPA and PA 212.
Modern Learning Theories and Mathematics Education: Bidirectional Contributions, Bidirectional Challenges The research reported here was supported by the.
1 Functions and Applications
‘She’s not a teacher, she’s a mummy’: reflections from four-year- olds on a researcher’s identity. Julie Evans College of St Mark and St John
9. Two Functions of Two Random Variables
1 Dr. Scott Schaefer Least Squares Curves, Rational Representations, Splines and Continuity.
Ashley David Sara McTaggart Annie Porter Ellen Ramsey
Principle B and Phonologically Reduced Pronouns in Child English Jeremy Hartman Yasutada Sudo Ken Wexler.
Gender effects in a randomized trial of individual tutoring with children in care Robyn Marquis & Robert J. Flynn School of Psychology & Centre for Research.
BBN-ANG-253 Advanced Syntax Lecture Course Autumn, 2014/15
Brian MacWhinney, CMU Heidi Feldman, Pitt Kelley Sacco, CMU
Safely Supporting the Development of the Visual Arts in Infants and Toddlers Shannon D. Lockhart
Observing Children in School. Aims To learn how to observe children in one of their natural habitats To understand how children experience their worlds.
Quantifier spreading: children misled by ostensive cues
. RESEARCH QUESTION LINGUISTIC BACKGROUND Experiment 1 Conclusions and Future Questions How do children learn different types of indefinites that are masked.
Children’s scope interpretation of doubly quantified sentences and the problem of isomorphism Katalin É. Kiss & Tamás Zétényi Research.
Data, Now What? Skills for Analyzing and Interpreting Data
© Imperial College LondonPage 1 Model checking and refinement checking for modal transition systems and their cousins MTS meeting 2007 Adam Antonik & Michael.
SNKYO A Study on Adult Working Memory Nate R. and Arin T.
Ling 240: Language and Mind Structure Dependence in Grammar Formation.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 2 Introduction to Linguistic Theory, Part 4.
„Ostention effect” in language acquisition experiments Katalin É. Kiss, Mátyás Gerőcs, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of.
Linguistic Theory Lecture 8 Meaning and Grammar. A brief history In classical and traditional grammar not much distinction was made between grammar and.
Focus affected quantification in adult and child langage Erik-Jan Smits Semantics in the Netherlands Day Utrecht University of Groningen, Dutch.
LEARNING FROM OBSERVATIONS Yılmaz KILIÇASLAN. Definition Learning takes place as the agent observes its interactions with the world and its own decision-making.
LEARNING FROM OBSERVATIONS Yılmaz KILIÇASLAN. Definition Learning takes place as the agent observes its interactions with the world and its own decision-making.
Domain restriction in child language Erik-Jan Smits 1, Tom Roeper 2 and Bart Hollebrandse 1 1 University of Groningen, The Netherlands 2 University of.
Emergence of Syntax. Introduction  One of the most important concerns of theoretical linguistics today represents the study of the acquisition of language.
An investigation of Conservativity Tim Hunter Anastasia Conroy.
What readings does a given sentence have? Some sentences containing a quantifier and negation are semantically ambiguous. They reveal two readings: Alle.
An experimental investigation of referential/non-referential asymmetries in syntactic reconstruction akira omaki anastasia conroy jeffrey lidz Quantitative.
Ostention effect in experiments testing children’s interpretation of quantification Katalin É. Kiss, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for.
A methodological problem of language acquisition studies Katalin É. Kiss, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of the Hungarian.
Chapter five.  Language is a communication tools whose development depends on the prior development of communication.  Language is a social tool.* 
When is children’s scope interpretation non-isomorphic, and why? Katalin É. Kiss & Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of.
Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference S. Shimojo, C. Simion, E. Shimojo, and Scheier 발표 : 생물심리 전공 설선혜.
 concepts  solve problems  make decisions.
Discovering the Arts  Elements of Music  Elements of Art.
Chapter 11 Language. Some Questions to Consider How do we understand individual words, and how are words combined to create sentences? How can we understand.
An Introduction to the Government and Binding Theory
Thinking & Language (Chapter 10)
Presentation transcript:

Hungarian preschoolers’ interpretation of doubly quantified sentences Katalin É. Kiss, Mátyás Gerőcs, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of the Hungarian Academy

Research question: how do Hungarian preschoolers interpret quanti- fied sentences with two numerical quantifiers, e.g. (1) Két fiú is három tornyot épít. two boy DIST three tower-ACC builds ’Two boys (each) are building three towers’.

Previous experiments involving truth value judgment, forced choice, and acting-out tasks: preschoolers have access to the distributive readings of doubly quantified sentences. Follow-up question: How do preschoolers determine relative scope?

What linguistic cues determin their scope choice? Do they associate sentence (1) with representation (a) or representation (b)? Fig.a: Fig.b: (1) Két fiú is három tornyot épít. two boys each three towers-ACC build

Claim: Children’s scope interpretation cannot be derived from linguistic factors alone, it is affected by the structure of the visual representation of the event associated with the sentence. In a forced choice task, they choose the representation that is easier to segment into identical subevents.

Background: Musolino (1998): children assume isomorphism between the scope order & linear order of QPs. Children have problems with inverse scope because i. they cannot generate the complex structure associated with inverse scope (Musolino 2000) ii. they cannot process such sentences (Musolino & Lidz 2003; Lidz et al. 2004) iii. Gennari &MacDonald (2005/2006): children’s behavior reflects the distributional patterns of actual language use

Contradictory results from Chinese: The scope interpretation of children is less isomorphic than the scope interpretation of adults – cf. Thomas Hun-tak Lee (1997), Zhou and Crain (2009)

Hungarian adult grammar: overt Quantifier-Raising, preverbal QPs c-command and precede their scope. Subject wide scope : (2) FocP 2 fiú is j FocP 3 tornyot k Foc′ Foc TenseP épít i Tense VP t i NP V′ t j V NP t i t k

(3) FocP 3 tornyot is k FocP 2 fiú i Foc′ Foc TenseP épít i Tense VP t i NP V′ t j V NP t i t k Object wide scope:

Scope interpretation by Hungarian preschoolers É. Kiss–Gerőcs (2011): Starting hypothesis: isomorphism: wide scope assigned on the basis of linear precedence

Hypotheses not borne out; scope preferences often cannot be derived from linguistic cues E.g. Truth-value judgement of sentence - picture pairs: (4) Három maci is két autóval játszik. three teddy-bear DIST two car-with plays ‘Three teddy bears each are playing with two cars.’

Conditions and results (acceptance rates): i. S > O (subj.-initial, direct scope): 91% (3 bears, 6 cars) ii. S < O (subj.-initial, inverse scope): 63% 6 bears, 2 cars) iii. O > S (obj.-initial, direct scope): 67% (2 cars, 6 bears) iv.O < S (obj.-initial, inverse scope): 41% (6 cars, 3 bears)

A follow-up experiment checking the role of further linguistic factors É. Kiss – Gerőcs – Zétényi (2012): Hypothesis: wide scope assigned to the constituent that has a more prominent grammatical function/theta role Forced choice, acting-out tasks Hypothesis again not fully borne out.

New hypotheses: children’s scope interpretation is affected by visual cues Hypothesis 1: children assign wide scope to the quantified expression that has larger, more salient denotata in the visual representation. Hypothesis 2: children choose the picture that is easier to divide into identical chunks representing identical subevents.

Experiment 1 Method Subjects: 27 children, 12 boys + 15 girls, mean age 6,5 years (SD=4 months) Materials: Subjects were shown 8 test sentences + 8 fillers, each accompanied by a pair of pictures. They had to decide which of the two pictures the sentence is about.

Conditions Condition i SOV sentence, with the subject set more salient Condition ii SOV sentence, with the object set more salient Condition iii OSV sentence, with the object set more salient Condition iv OSV sentence, with the subject set more salient

E.g., Condition iii: OSV sentence, with the object set more salient Fig.a: Fig.b: (5) Három tornyot is két fiú épít. three tower- ACC DIST two boy builds ‘Three towers(each), two boys are building.’

Condition iv: OSV sentence, with the subject set more salient Fig.a: Fig.b: (6) Három tornyot is két fiú épít. three tower- ACC DIST two boy builds ‘Three towers (each) are being built by two boys.’

Results in conditions iii, iv (7)b. Három tornyot is két fiú épít. ‘Three towers are being built by two boys.’ Condition iii. big towers, small boys: direct scope (3 big towers, 6 small boys): 33% inverse scope (6 big towers, 2 small boys): 67% Condition iv. small towers, big boys: direct scope (3 small towers, 6 big boys): 30% inverse scope (6 small towers, 2 big boys): 70% Hypothesis not borne out.

Experiment 2 Method Subjects: 38 children, 18 boys + 20 girls, mean age 6,5 (SD=4 months) Materials: Subjects were shown 8 test sentences + 7 fillers. Test sentences were accompanied by pairs of pictures, showing their direct & inverse scopes. They had to decide which of the two pictures the sentence is about.

One member of each picture pair was chunked into identical subevents separated by spaces. In the other picture, the members of the two sets were mixed.

Conditions: Condition i SOV, with the direct scope representation chunked Condition ii SOV, with the inverse scope representation chunked Condition iii OSV, with the direct scope representation chunked Condition iv OSV, with the inverse scope representation chunked

E.g., Condition i: SOV, with the direct scope representation chunked Fig.a: Fig.b: Fig. 14a (7) Három lány is két virágot locsol. three girl DIST two flower waters ’Three girls (each) are watering two flowers.’

E.g., Condition ii: SOV, with the inverse scope representation chunked Fig.a: Fig.b: Fig. 18a. 18b (7) Három lány is két virágot locsol. three girl DIST two flower waters ’Three girls (each) are watering two flowers.’

Results: Preference for scope reading with a chunked visual representation: Condition i: SOV, chunked direct scope: 66% Condition ii: SOV, chunked inverse scope: 29% Condition iii: OSV, chunked direct scope: 58% Condition iv: OSV, chunked inverse scope: 71%

Hypothesis confirmed in conditions i, iii, iv. What about condition ii? The visual stimuli may not have been sufficiently different. (8) Két markoló is három gödröt ás. two excavator DIST three hole-ACC digs ’Two excavators (each) are digging three holes.’

Fig. a: Fig. b: (8) Két markoló is három gödröt ás. two excavator DIST three hole-ACC digs

(9) Két fiú is három tornyot épít. two boy DIST three tower builds ’Two boys (each) are building three towers.’ Fig.a: Fig.b: Fig. 15a Fig. 15b

(8), (9): wide scope assigned to the initial, subject quantifier When both pictures are easy to chunk into identical subevents, linguistic prominence factors decide scope preferences.

Conclusion: The primary factor determining children’s preferred scope reading is the structure of the visual representation of the event. Linguistic cues play a secondary role; the majority of subjects assign wide scope to the linguistically more prominent quantifier only if both visual representations are segmentable into identical subevents.

More generally: Whereas adults processing a doubly quantified sentence have no problems in mentally reconstructing the complex situation presented linguistically, preschoolers also exploit visual cues.  Their processing abilities are still immature.

Eye tracking confirms that chunked visual information is easier to process: Heat map: Multiple eye-movements when observing mixed representations; as many brief gazes as the number of subevents when observing chunked representations. Much more time is spent on mixed representations than on chunked ones.

Chunking also helps infants to interpretat visual stimuli Feigenson & Halberda (2004): Infants can distinguish sets of individuals larger than 3 if the sets are chunked.

Chunking has also been claimed to play a role in the interpretation of universal quantifiers: Philip (1995), Brooks & Braine (1996), Braine & Sekerina (1996), etc.: Universal quantification in child grammar is quantification over (sub)events rather than individuals.

Quantification over (sub)-events is more elementary than quantification over individuals Bach, Jelinek, Kratzer and Partee (1995): A-quantification is primary to D-quantification. A-quantification is universal, D-quantification is not – cf. Mohawk, Navajo, Lakhota, Straits Salish. Languages with deficient number systems: the Amazonian Mundurucú, the Australian Warlpiri, Mayali, Gun-djeyhmi, only have A-quantification.