Making sense of the maze: Exploring the source of neologistic errors in a case of jargon aphasia Melanie Moses 1,2,3, Lyndsey Nickels 2, Christine Sheard.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Core Features of Episodic Memory l (1) Memory for specific events from your past l (2) Involves retrieving the bound together contents and context (what.
Advertisements

Cognitive models of spelling and writing Types of dysgraphia
Associate Professor Lyndsey Nickels
Associate Professor Lyndsey Nickels
Is errorless learning a useful concept in the treatment of word retrieval disorders? Lyndsey Nickels, Kate Makin, Belinda McDonald Melanie Moses & Christine.
Levels of breakdown in impaired word retrieval
ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP 2008 Extravaganza ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP Anika Roseby and Kate Schuj Group Co- Leaders.
What phonological deficit?
Psycholinguistic what is psycholinguistic? 1-pyscholinguistic is the study of the cognitive process of language acquisition and use. 2-The scope of psycholinguistic.
Working Memory Dr. Claudia J. Stanny EXP 4507 Memory & Cognition Spring 2009.
Phonological dyslexia Introduction Phonological dyslexia was first described by Beauvois and Derouesne (1979); other early case are in Shallice and Warrington.
Examining the Relationship Between Confrontational Naming Tasks & Discourse Production in Aphasia Leila D. Luna & Gerasimos Fergadiotis Portland State.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
Language and Cognition Colombo 2011 Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia – Word comprehension With acknowledgement to Jane Marshall.
Disorders of Lexical Selection Garret 1992b Brian Nisonger.
Aphasia A disorder caused by damage to the parts of the brain that control language. It can make it hard to read, or write and to comprehend or produce.
Introduction Semantic Feature Analysis (SFA) is a treatment technique designed to improve the naming abilities by increasing the level of activation within.
Language Disorders October 12, Types of Disorders Aphasia: acquired disorder of language due to brain damage Dysarthria: disorder of motor apparatus.
Language and Brain. Is Language Situated in our Brain? Neurolinguistics is the study of how the brain processes language.
Language Ref: Banich pp Broca's Aphasia: Typical Features Slowed, effortful speech, with many pauses Slowed, effortful speech, with many pauses.
Communication after a Stroke
MossTalk Training for Word Retrieval Across Semantic Categories Tiffany Johnson, Erin Todd, & Anastasia Raymer* Old Dominion University, Norfolk VA; *Brain.
Cognitive Psychology, 2 nd Ed. Chapter 11 Language Production.
Experiment 2: MEG Study Materials and Methods: 11 right-handed subjects with 20:20 vision were run. 3 subjects’ data was discarded because of poor performance.
Models of Language Language and Cognition Colombo 2011.
Profile of Phoneme Auditory Perception Ability in Children with Hearing Impairment and Phonological Disorders By Manal Mohamed El-Banna (MD) Unit of Phoniatrics,
Developmental Dyslexia By: Malin Kinberg, Neurolinguistics, Spring term 2007 Developmental dyslexia – specific reading difficulties Developmental dyslexia.
Language. Using Language What is language for? Using Language What is language for? – Rapid, efficient communication To accomplish this goal, what needs.
Non-Word Repetition Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.
Word Retrieval in a Stem Completion Task: Influence of Number of Potential Responses Christine Chiarello 1, Laura K. Halderman 1, Cathy S. Robinson 1 &
Language processing What are the components of language, and how do we process them?
Reading. Reading Research Processes involved in reading –Orthography (the spelling of words) –Phonology (the sound of words) –Word meaning –Syntax –Higher-level.
Aphasias: Language Disturbances Associated with Brain Injury The Classic View: based on symptoms and associated with particular brain areas The Major Syndromes:
Language and Cognition Colombo 2011 Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia - Writing With acknowledgement to Jane Marshall.
APHASIA. What is Aphasia? Aphasia is a total or partial loss of the ability to use words.
Despite adjustments to the Wernicke-Lichtheim model, there remained disorders which could not be explained. Later models (e.g., Heilman’s) have included.
Main Branches of Linguistics
Language and Cognition Colombo June 2011 Day 5 Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia Producing Words Acknowledgement to Jane Marshall.
1 Language disorders We can learn a lot by looking at system failure –Which parts are connected to which Examine the relation between listening/speaking.
BDAE: Acoustic Comprehension Scores
1 ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN TYPICALLY DEVELOPING CHILDREN’S COMPLEX SENTENCE COMPREHENSION AUTHORS; Shwetha M.P.,Deepthi M. Trupthi T, Nikhil Mathur &
Introduction Pinker and colleagues (Pinker & Ullman, 2002) have argued that morphologically irregular verbs must be stored as full forms in the mental.
+ Treatment of Aphasia Week 12 April 1 st, Review Involvement of semantic and phonological stages in naming. Differentiating features of naming.
Last Lecture Dichotic Listening Dichotic Listening The corpus callosum & resource allocation The corpus callosum & resource allocation Handedness Handedness.
Reading disorders in mental retardation. Dyslexia or not ? Annick COMBLAIN, University of Liege – FAPSE Department of Cognitive Sciences Speech and language.
CSD 2230 HUMAN COMMUNICATION DISORDERS Topic 6 Language Disorders Adult Disorders Aphasia and Right Hemisphere Injury.
Assessment of Aphasia.
Language and Cognition Colombo 2011 Day 7 Specific Issues in Aphasia – Treatments for production impairments.
The Influence of Feature Type, Feature Structure and Psycholinguistic Parameters on the Naming Performance of Semantic Dementia and Alzheimer’s Patients.
+ Treatment of Aphasia Week 10 March 17 th, 2011.
Semantic Processing and Irregularly Inflected Forms Michele Miozzo & Peter Gordon Columbia University Introduction Recent models of lexical representation.
Late Talkers Phoniatric Dept., 1st Faculty of Medicine Charles University Prague, Czech Republic O. Dlouhá.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011
Lexical and morphosyntactic minimal pairs. Evidence for different processing Luca Cilibrasi, Vesna Stojanovik, Patricia Riddell, School of Psychology,
The Edinburgh Disfluency Group Researching disfluency from a psycholinguistic perspective: Language.
Comparing the effectiveness of orthographic and phonological cues in the treatment of anomia. Lyndsey Nickels 1, Antje Lorenz 1,2, 1 Macquarie Centre for.
The Cross-Script Length Effect: Evidence for Serial Processing in Reading Aloud Kathleen Rastle (Royal Holloway University of London), Linda Bayliss (Royal.
ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP Extravaganza 2007 Anika Hobbs and Kate Schuj Group Co-Leaders.
Heilmann, J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J. and C. Hollar. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventory in Identifying Language.
Acknowledgments Research Mentor: Catherine Off, Ph.D. Graduate Student Mentor: Jenna Griffin Neuroplasticity, Dosage, and Repetition Priming Effects in.
VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION. What is Word Recognition? Features, letters & word interactions Interactive Activation Model Lexical and Sublexical Approach.
Constraints on definite article alternation in speech production: To “thee” or not to “thee”? By M. GARETH GASKELL, HELEN COX, KATHERINE FOLEY, HELEN GRIEVE,
Why spoken language development is considered an important foundation for written language and how this information would be used for planning interventions.
Chapter 8 Reading and Writing
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition
Levels of Processing Memory Model (LoP)
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition
2008 Extravaganza ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP
How precise are verbal working memory representations
Presentation transcript:

Making sense of the maze: Exploring the source of neologistic errors in a case of jargon aphasia Melanie Moses 1,2,3, Lyndsey Nickels 2, Christine Sheard 3 Royal Rehabilitation Centre Sydney 1, Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science, Macquarie University 2, The University of Sydney 3

Neologisms Typify language in jargon aphasia Disagreement re definition and source Different definitions:  any nonword response (e.g Miller and Ellis, 1987)  unrelated to target Vs phonologically-related (e.g. Buckingham, 1987; Schwartz, et al., 1994)

This presentation…. Neologism = nonword responses that are unrelated to target. e.g. ball  dEb  Non-word responses phonologically related to the target = phonological errors. e.g. ball  bIl 

Case Study: KVH 71-year-old-man Left basal ganglia (CVA) in January 2000 Severe fluent jargon aphasia. Wernickes  Conduction Aphasia Fluent spontaneous speech with ++ perseverative, neologistic & semantic jargon Good comprehension at basic conversational level but difficulties at complex level

Aims Determine KVH’s language processing breakdown Determine the source of KVH’s neologisms

Phonological Output Lexicon Speech Phonological Output Buffer Phonological encoding Phonological Input Lexicon Phonological Input Buffer Acoustic-to-phonological conversion Auditory analysis Speech X Mild impairment X Mild impairment X But many phonologically- related errors  can process some phonological information

Conceptual Semantics Lexical Semantics Visual Object Recognition System Phonological Input Lexicon Phonological Input Buffer Acoustic-to-phonological conversion Auditory analysis Orthographic Input Lexicon Abstract Letter Identification Visual feature analysis Speech Print Pictures, seen objects X X X Moderate central semantic deficit

Phonological Output Lexicon Speech Phonological Output Buffer Phonological encoding Lexical Semantics Orthographic Input Lexicon Abstract Letter Identification Letter-sound rules Visual feature analysis Print Pictures, seen objects X X X X More phonologically-related responses to nonwords & regular words  some intact sublexical processing X Severely impaired access to phonological form via lexical reading route

Research Tasks Picture naming, word reading aloud, word repetition 126 items, presented twice Repetition: –few errors, mainly phonological (real & nonword)  mild phonological encoding difficulties –few neologisms Naming & Reading Aloud: –many errors  50% neologistic –large proportion of phonological errors in reading reflects  impaired phonological encoding –imageability effect in naming (Wald = 4.818; p =.028)  semantic impairment.

Where is KVH’s language breaking down?

Phonological Output Lexicon Speech Phonological Output Buffer Phonological encoding Conceptual Semantics Lexical Semantics Visual Object Recognition System Orthographic Output Lexicon Graphemic Output Buffer Writing Phonological Input Lexicon Phonological Input Buffer Acoustic-to-phonological conversion Sound-Letter Rules Auditory analysis Orthographic Input Lexicon Abstract Letter Identification Letter-sound rules Visual feature analysis Speech Print Pictures, seen objects

Where do KVH’s neologisms come from? Let’s first look at the literature…….

Impaired self-monitoring? Poor self-awareness of speech errors in jargon aphasia (Marshall et al., 1998)  more susceptible to neologisms Poor self-monitoring linked with poor auditory comprehension (Ellis et al., 1983) although this is debatable (Nickels & Howard, 1995)

Can impaired self-monitoring account for KVH’s neologisms? Superior self-monitoring in repetition (least errors, few neologisms):  proportionately more errors rejected (Vs. naming or reading)  more likely to reject error than correct response  largest proportion of “don’t know” responses  presence of phonological model in repetition to compare intended with actual response?

But….. In repetition:  just as likely to reattempt a correct as error response and unable to successfully self-correct errors.  reattempted only 20% of errors, only 1 resulting in correct response In picture naming:  many neologisms  significantly more error than correct responses reattempted  more accurate self-monitoring than repetition?  Relationship between neologisms & self-monitoring not straightforward KVH’s neologisms can’t be explained in terms of poor self-monitoring alone.

Impaired phonological encoding? Neologisms reflect severe distortion of a target at phonological encoding level  response contains no target-related phonemes? (e.g. Kertesz & Benson, 1970) Phonological distortion of an error from an earlier stage of lexical access (e.g. Nickels, 2001) (semantic error  phonological error)

Can impaired phonological encoding account for KVH’s neologisms? Could account for the source of some of KVH’s neologisms BUT... he should have produced large numbers of neologisms in repetition as phonological encoding is common to all 3 tasks absence of syllable length effects in any task  primary source of KVH’s neologisms is NOT phonological encoding impairment

Underlying lexical access impairment? Neologisms fill in a “lexical” gap when word selection fails (Buckingham & Kertesz, 1976; Butterworth, 1979, 1992). Butterworth (1979, 1992) proposed “KC” used back-up “device” which generates neologisms after failure to retrieve lexical target. neologisms generated by random assembly of previously produced phonemes – ie. perseveration Obeyed English phonotactic rules Didn’t obey English phoneme frequency x = no underlying lexical target?

Butterworth (1979, 1992) Neologistic errors reflected failed attempt to retrieve the target word at lexical level  default to a neologism-generating “device.” Phonemic variants of a “device” neologism may be used up to 5 or 6 times  string of phonologically similar neologistic responses. Example: b  kl  nd – b  ndIks –  ndIks – z  ndIks – l  ndIks – z  prIks These phonologically-related neologisms are well documented in jargon aphasia

Can impaired lexical access account for KVH’s neologisms? Neologisms may result from severe impairment in accessing the lexical form of the word. Naming = SS  POL X Reading aloud = OIL  SS  POL X Phonological encoding deficits further impact on performance Can access sublexical phonological information in repetition Unable to derive sublexical phonological information from written input

Therefore... insufficient activation of target lexical representation  phonemes from previous responses assembled to form a neologism. neologism fills the lexical “slot” for the missing target (Butterworth, 1979; 1992)  KVH’s neologisms could reflect an underlying impairment accessing the lexical form of the word via both spoken or written modalities.

Perseverative influence on neologisms Majority of KVH’s neologistic errors in all tasks were perseverative (Repetition: 67%; Reading: 83%; Naming 64%). Suggests production of neologisms strongly linked to a process of perseveration

KVH’s perseverative error patterns KVH mainly produced phoneme perseverations in all tasks But different types in Repetition Vs Picture Naming & Reading Aloud

Repetition  Nail  n1l  Star  st1l Short duration, phonologically related to target

Neologistic perseverative strings Picture naming:  p  s  n  pIs  pI  s  pI  s  n  f  r  n pI  (bowl) (glasses) (carrot) (desk) (cannon) Reading aloud:  sibr   sig   sua  sup   sug  (zebra) (chain) (apple) (carrot) (mountain) Long duration, unrelated to target Consistent with neologistic strings in literature on jargon aphasia

KVH’s perseverative errors KVH’s perseverative errors reflect his different levels of processing breakdown, (phonological encoding in repetition, lexical access in reading aloud and picture naming) Consistent with recent research on perseveration (Cohen & Dehaene, 1998, Martin et al., 1998, Moses et al 2004, Hirsh, 1998)

Conclusions KVH’s neologisms most likely reflect impaired activation of phonological forms via the semantic system  Consistent with some research (e.g. Butterworth, 1979, 1992; Simmons and Buckingham, 1992)  Contradicts others proposing neologisms reflect severe underlying phonological encoding difficulties alone (e.g. Kertesz and Benson, 1970; Lecours and Lhermitte, 1969) KVH’s neologisms typical of jargon aphasia Errors are consistent with Butterworth’s (1979, 1992) neologism generator theory Strong link between KVH’s production of neologisms and phoneme perseveration

Future Directions Investigate alternative accounts for production of neologisms e.g. substitution of phonemes based on phoneme frequency (Butterworth, 1992) More detailed discussion of nature of KVH’s perseverative errors and links with neologisms Replication across series of individuals with jargon aphasia

References Buckingham HW. Perseveration in aphasia. In: Newman S, Epstein R, editors. Current perspectives in dysphasia. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingston, 1985:113–54. Buckingham HW. Phonemic paraphasias and psycholinguistic production models for neologistic jargon. Aphasiology 1987; 1: 381–400. Buckingham HW, Jr, Kertesz A. Neologistic jargon aphasia. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger; Buckingham HW Jr, Whitaker HA, Whitaker, H.A. Alliteration and assonance in neologistic jargon aphasia. Cortex 1978; 14: 365–80. Buckingham HW Jr, Whitaker HA, Whitaker HA. On linguistic perseveration. Studies in Neurolinguistics 1979; 4: 329–35. Butterworth B. Hesitation and the production of verbal paraphasias and neologisms in jargon aphasia. Brain and Language 1979; 8: 133–61. Butterworth B. Disorders of phonological encoding. Cognition 1992; 42: 261–86. Cohen L, Dehaene S. Competition between past and present: Assessment and interpretation of verbal perseverations. Brain 1998; 121: 1641–59. Hirsh KW. Perseveration and activation in aphasic speech production.Cognitive Neuropsychology 1998; 15: 377–88. Kertesz A, Benson DF. Neologistic jargon: A clinico-pathological study. Cortex 1970; 6: 362–86. Lecours AR, Lhermitte F. Phonemic paraphasias: Linguistic structures and tentative hypotheses. Cortex 1969; 5: 193–228. Martin N, Roach A, Brecher A, Lowery J. Lexical retrieval mechanisms underlying whole- word perseveration errors in anomic aphasia. Aphasiology 1998; 12: 319–33.

References (cont’d) Marshall, J., Robson, J, Pring, T, Chiat, S. (1998) Why does monitoring fail in jargon aphasia (1998). Comprehension, judgement and therapy evidence. Brain and Language, 63, Miller D, Ellis A. Speech and writing errors in “neologistic jargonaphasia”: A lexical activation hypothesis. In: Coltheart M, Job R, Sartori G, editors. The cognitive neuropsychology of language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1987; 253–70. Moses, M.S., Nickels, L.A. and Sheard, C. (2004). Disentangling the web. Neologistic perseverative errors in jargon aphasia., Neurocase, 10 (6), Nickels L. Words fail me: Symptoms and causes of naming breakdown in aphasia. In: Berndt RS, editor. Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd edition (vol. 3). Language and Aphasia. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2001; p. 115–35. Nickels LA. A sketch of the cognitive processes involved in the comprehension and production of single words, Retrieved 1/6/04 from Schwartz MF, Saffran, EM, Bloch DE, Dell G. Disordered speech production in aphasic and normal speakers. Brain and Language 1994; 47: 52–88. Simmons N, Buckingham HW. Recovery in jargon aphasia, Aphasiology, 1992; 6: 403–14. Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M. A standardised set of 260 pictures: Normals for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1980; 6: 174–215.