Pitfalls of Hypothesis Testing + Sample Size Calculations.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Introduction to Hypothesis Testing
Advertisements

Statistics 101 Class 8. Overview Hypothesis Testing Hypothesis Testing Stating the Research Question Stating the Research Question –Null Hypothesis –Alternative.
Statistical Inference June 30-July 1, 2004 Statistical Inference The process of making guesses about the truth from a sample. Sample (observation) Make.
Hypothesis Testing Steps of a Statistical Significance Test. 1. Assumptions Type of data, form of population, method of sampling, sample size.
Hypothesis : Statement about a parameter Hypothesis testing : decision making procedure about the hypothesis Null hypothesis : the main hypothesis H 0.
C82MCP Diploma Statistics School of Psychology University of Nottingham 1 Overview of Lecture Independent and Dependent Variables Between and Within Designs.
Hypothesis Testing for the Mean and Variance of a Population Introduction to Business Statistics, 5e Kvanli/Guynes/Pavur (c)2000 South-Western College.
BCOR 1020 Business Statistics Lecture 20 – April 3, 2008.
Business Statistics - QBM117 Testing hypotheses about a population mean.
Determining Statistical Significance
© 2002 Thomson / South-Western Slide 9-1 Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing with Single Samples.
Hypothesis Testing – Introduction
Hypothesis Testing.
Chapter 8 Introduction to Hypothesis Testing
1/2555 สมศักดิ์ ศิวดำรงพงศ์
7 Elementary Statistics Hypothesis Testing. Introduction to Hypothesis Testing Section 7.1.
1 Power and Sample Size in Testing One Mean. 2 Type I & Type II Error Type I Error: reject the null hypothesis when it is true. The probability of a Type.
Chapter 8 Hypothesis Testing I. Chapter Outline  An Overview of Hypothesis Testing  The Five-Step Model for Hypothesis Testing  One-Tailed and Two-Tailed.
Hypothesis Testing: One Sample Cases. Outline: – The logic of hypothesis testing – The Five-Step Model – Hypothesis testing for single sample means (z.
Statistical Hypotheses & Hypothesis Testing. Statistical Hypotheses There are two types of statistical hypotheses. Null Hypothesis The null hypothesis,
Chapter 8 Introduction to Hypothesis Testing ©. Chapter 8 - Chapter Outcomes After studying the material in this chapter, you should be able to: 4 Formulate.
Inferential Statistics Body of statistical computations relevant to making inferences from findings based on sample observations to some larger population.
Copyright © 2013 Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Prentice Hall Statistics for Business and Economics 8 th Edition Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing: Single.
Chapter 8 Hypothesis Testing I. Significant Differences  Hypothesis testing is designed to detect significant differences: differences that did not occur.
Inen 460 Lecture 2. Estimation (ch. 6,7) and Hypothesis Testing (ch.8) Two Important Aspects of Statistical Inference Point Estimation – Estimate an unknown.
Hypothesis Testing Errors. Hypothesis Testing Suppose we believe the average systolic blood pressure of healthy adults is normally distributed with mean.
Introduction to hypothesis testing Hypothesis testing is about making decisions Is a hypothesis true or false? Ex. Are women paid less, on average, than.
STEP BY STEP Critical Value Approach to Hypothesis Testing 1- State H o and H 1 2- Choose level of significance, α Choose the sample size, n 3- Determine.
Introduction to Hypothesis Testing
Inference as Design Target Goal: I can calculate and interpret a type I and type II error. 9.1c h.w: pg 547: 15, 19, 21.
Sampling Distributions Statistics Introduction Let’s assume that the IQ in the population has a mean (  ) of 100 and a standard deviation (  )
Hypothesis Testing Steps for the Rejection Region Method State H 1 and State H 0 State the Test Statistic and its sampling distribution (normal or t) Determine.
Chapter 9: Hypothesis Tests for One Population Mean 9.2 Terms, Errors, and Hypotheses.
More about tests and intervals CHAPTER 21. Do not state your claim as the null hypothesis, instead make what you’re trying to prove the alternative. The.
Ex St 801 Statistical Methods Part 2 Inference about a Single Population Mean (HYP)
+ Homework 9.1:1-8, 21 & 22 Reading Guide 9.2 Section 9.1 Significance Tests: The Basics.
INTRODUCTION TO TESTING OF HYPOTHESIS INTRODUCTION TO TESTING OF HYPOTHESIS SHWETA MOGRE.
Warm Up #’s 12, 14, and 16 on p. 552 Then answer the following question; In a jury trial, what two errors could a jury make?
CHAPTER 9 Testing a Claim
Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing.
Dr. Amjad El-Shanti MD, PMH,Dr PH University of Palestine 2016
8-1 of 23.
Hypothesis Testing II: The Two-sample Case
Hypothesis Testing – Introduction
Hypothesis Testing: Hypotheses
MATH 2311 Section 8.2.
CONCEPTS OF HYPOTHESIS TESTING
Introduction to Inference
A Closer Look at Testing
P-value Approach for Test Conclusion
CHAPTER 9 Testing a Claim
Hypothesis Testing (Cont.)
Chapter 9: Hypothesis Testing
Introduction to Inference
AP Statistics: Chapter 21
Statistical inference
Chapter 9: Hypothesis Tests Based on a Single Sample
Statistical Inference for Managers
Chapter 11: Introduction to Hypothesis Testing Lecture 5c
Inference Concepts Hypothesis Testing.
Sample Mean Compared to a Given Population Mean
Sample Mean Compared to a Given Population Mean
CHAPTER 9 Testing a Claim
Chapter 9: Testing a Claim
CHAPTER 9 Testing a Claim
The POWER of a hypothesis test
Inference as Decision Section 10.4.
Power and Error What is it?.
Inference about Population Mean
Statistical inference
Presentation transcript:

Pitfalls of Hypothesis Testing + Sample Size Calculations

Hypothesis Testing The Steps: 1. Define your hypotheses (null, alternative) 2. Specify your null distribution 3. Do an experiment 4. Calculate the p-value of what you observed 5. Reject or fail to reject (~accept) the null hypothesis Follows the logic: If A then B; not B; therefore, not A.

Summary: The Underlying Logic of hypothesis tests… Follows this logic: Assume A. If A, then B. Not B. Therefore, Not A. But throw in a bit of uncertainty…If A, then probably B…

Error and Power Type-I Error (also known as “ α” ): Rejecting the null when the effect isn’t real. Type-II Error (also known as “β “): Failing to reject the null when the effect is real. POWER (the flip side of type-II error: 1- β): The probability of seeing a true effect if one exists. Note the sneaky conditionals…

Think of… Pascal’s Wager Your Decision The TRUTH God ExistsGod Doesn’t Exist Reject God BIG MISTAKECorrect Accept God Correct— Big Pay Off MINOR MISTAKE

Type I and Type II Error in a box Your Statistical Decision True state of null hypothesis H 0 True (example: the drug doesn’t work) H 0 False (example: the drug works) Reject H 0 (ex: you conclude that the drug works) Type I error (α)Correct Do not reject H 0 (ex: you conclude that there is insufficient evidence that the drug works) Correct Type II Error (β)

Error and Power Type I error rate (or significance level): the probability of finding an effect that isn’t real (false positive). If we require p-value<.05 for statistical significance, this means that 1/20 times we will find a positive result just by chance. Type II error rate: the probability of missing an effect (false negative). Statistical power: the probability of finding an effect if it is there (the probability of not making a type II error). When we design studies, we typically aim for a power of 80% (allowing a false negative rate, or type II error rate, of 20%).

Pitfall 1: over-emphasis on p- values Clinically unimportant effects may be statistically significant if a study is large (and therefore, has a small standard error and extreme precision). Pay attention to effect size and confidence intervals.

Example: effect size A prospective cohort study of 34,079 women found that women who exercised >21 MET hours per week gained significantly less weight than women who exercised <7.5 MET hours (p<.001) Headlines: “To Stay Trim, Women Need an Hour of Exercise Daily.” Physical Activity and Weight Gain Prevention. JAMA 2010;303:

Copyright restrictions may apply. Lee, I. M. et al. JAMA 2010;303: Mean (SD) Differences in Weight Over Any 3-Year Period by Physical Activity Level, Women's Health Study, a

What was the effect size? Those who exercised the least 0.15 kg (.33 pounds) more than those who exercised the most over 3 years. Extrapolated over 13 years of the study, the high exercisers gained 1.4 pounds less than the low exercisers! Classic example of a statistically significant effect that is not clinically significant.

A picture is worth…

But baseline physical activity should predict weight gain in the first three years…do those slopes look different to you? Authors explain: “Figure 2 shows the trajectory of weight gain over time by baseline physical activity levels. When classified by this single measure of physical activity, all 3 groups showed similar weight gain patterns over time.”

Another recent headline Drinkers May Exercise More Than Teetotalers Activity levels rise along with alcohol use, survey shows “MONDAY, Aug. 31 (HealthDay News) -- Here's something to toast: Drinkers are often exercisers”… “In reaching their conclusions, the researchers examined data from participants in the 2005 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System, a yearly telephone survey of about 230,000 Americans.”… For women, those who imbibed exercised 7.2 minutes more per week than teetotalers. The results applied equally to men…

Pitfall 2: association does not equal causation Statistical significance does not imply a cause-effect relationship. Interpret results in the context of the study design.

A significance level of 0.05 means that your false positive rate for one test is 5%. If you run more than one test, your false positive rate will be higher than 5%. Pitfall 3: multiple comparisons

data dredging/multiple comparisons In 1980, researchers at Duke randomized 1073 heart disease patients into two groups, but treated the groups equally. Not surprisingly, there was no difference in survival. Then they divided the patients into 18 subgroups based on prognostic factors. In a subgroup of 397 patients (with three-vessel disease and an abnormal left ventricular contraction) survival of those in “group 1” was significantly different from survival of those in “group 2” (p<.025). How could this be since there was no treatment? (Lee et al. “Clinical judgment and statistics: lessons from a simulated randomized trial in coronary artery disease,” Circulation, 61: , 1980.)

The difference resulted from the combined effect of small imbalances in the subgroups Multiple comparisons

By using a p-value of 0.05 as the criterion for significance, we’re accepting a 5% chance of a false positive (of calling a difference significant when it really isn’t). If we compare survival of “treatment” and “control” within each of 18 subgroups, that’s 18 comparisons. If these comparisons were independent, the chance of at least one false positive would be…

Multiple comparisons With 18 independent comparisons, we have 60% chance of at least 1 false positive.

Multiple comparisons With 18 independent comparisons, we expect about 1 false positive.

Results from a previous class survey… My research question was to test whether or not being born on odd or even days predicted anything about people’s futures. I discovered that people who born on odd days got up later and drank more alcohol than people born on even days; they also had a trend of doing more homework (p=.04, p<.01, p=.09). Those born on odd days woke up 42 minutes later (7:48 vs. 7:06 am); drank 2.6 more drinks per week (1.1 vs. 3.7); and did 8 more hours of homework (22 hrs/week vs. 14).

Results from Class survey… I can see the NEJM article title now… “Being born on odd days predisposes you to alcoholism and laziness, but makes you a better med student.”

Results from Class survey… Assuming that this difference can’t be explained by astrology, it’s obviously an artifact! What’s going on?…

Results from Class survey… After the odd/even day question, I asked 25 other questions… I ran 25 statistical tests (comparing the outcome variable between odd-day born people and even-day born people). So, there was a high chance of finding at least one false positive!

P-value distribution for the 25 tests… Recall: Under the null hypothesis of no associations (which we’ll assume is true here!), p- values follow a uniform distribution… My significant p- values!

Compare with… Next, I generated 25 “p-values” from a random number generator (uniform distribution). These were the results from three runs…

In the medical literature… Researchers examined the relationship between intakes of caffeine/coffee/tea and breast cancer overall and in multiple subgroups (50 tests) Overall, there was no association Risk ratios were close to 1.0 (ranging from 0.67 to 1.79), indicated protection ( 1.0), and showed no consistent dose-response pattern But they found 4 “significant” p-values in subgroups: coffee intake was linked to increased risk in those with benign breast disease (p=.08) caffeine intake was linked to increased risk of estrogen/progesterone negative tumors and tumors larger than 2 cm (p=.02) decaf coffee was linked to reduced risk of BC in postmenopausal hormone users (p=.02) Ishitani K, Lin J, PhD, Manson JE, Buring JE, Zhang SM. Caffeine consumption and the risk of breast cancer in a large prospective cohort of women. Arch Intern Med. 2008;168:

Distribution of the p-values from the 50 tests Likely chance findings! Also, effect sizes showed no consistent pattern. The risk ratios: -were close to 1.0 (ranging from 0.67 to 1.79) -indicated protection ( 1.0) -showed no consistent dose- response pattern.

Hallmarks of a chance finding: Analyses are exploratory Many tests have been performed but only a few are significant The significant p-values are modest in size (between p=0.01 and p=0.05) The pattern of effect sizes is inconsistent The p-values are not adjusted for multiple comparisons

Conclusions Look at the totality of the evidence. Expect about one marginally significant p-value (.01<p<.05) for every 20 tests run. Be wary of unplanned comparisons (e.g., subgroup analyses).

Pitfall 4: high type II error (low statistical power) Results that are not statistically significant should not be interpreted as "evidence of no effect,” but as “no evidence of effect” Studies may miss effects if they are insufficiently powered (lack precision). Example: A study of 36 postmenopausal women failed to find a significant relationship between hormone replacement therapy and prevention of vertebral fracture. The odds ratio and 95% CI were: 0.38 (0.12, 1.19), indicating a potentially meaningful clinical effect. Failure to find an effect may have been due to insufficient statistical power for this endpoint. Ref: Wimalawansa et al. Am J Med 1998, 104:

Example “There was no significant effect of treatment (p =0.058), nor treatment by velocity interaction (p = 0.19), indicating that the treatment and control groups did not differ in their ability to perform the task.” P-values >.05 indicate that we have insufficient evidence of an effect; they do not constitute proof of no effect.

Smoking cessation trial Weight-concerned women smokers were randomly assigned to one of four groups: Weight-focused or standard counseling plus bupropion or placebo Outcome: biochemically confirmed smoking abstinence Levine MD, Perkins KS, Kalarchian MA, et al. Bupropion and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Weight-Concerned Women Smokers. Arch Intern Med 2010;170:

Rates of biochemically verified prolonged abstinence at 3, 6, and 12 months from a four-arm randomized trial of smoking cessation * Months after quit target date Weight-focused counselingStandard counseling group Bupropion group (n=106) Placebo group (n=87) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo Bupropion group (n=89) Placebo group (n=67) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo 341%18%.00133%19% %11%.00121%10% %8%.00619%7%.05 The Results…

Rates of biochemically verified prolonged abstinence at 3, 6, and 12 months from a four-arm randomized trial of smoking cessation * Months after quit target date Weight-focused counselingStandard counseling group Bupropion group (n=106) Placebo group (n=87) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo Bupropion group (n=89) Placebo group (n=67) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo 341%18%.00133%19% %11%.00121%10% %8%.00619%7%.05 The Results… Counseling methods appear equally effective in the placebo group.

Rates of biochemically verified prolonged abstinence at 3, 6, and 12 months from a four-arm randomized trial of smoking cessation * Months after quit target date Weight-focused counselingStandard counseling group Bupropion group (n=106) Placebo group (n=87) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo Bupropion group (n=89) Placebo group (n=67) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo 341%18%.00133%19% %11%.00121%10% %8%.00619%7%.05 The Results… Clearly, bupropion improves quitting rates in the weight-focused counseling group.

Rates of biochemically verified prolonged abstinence at 3, 6, and 12 months from a four-arm randomized trial of smoking cessation * Months after quit target date Weight-focused counselingStandard counseling group Bupropion group (n=106) Placebo group (n=87) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo Bupropion group (n=89) Placebo group (n=67) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo 341%18%.00133%19% %11%.00121%10% %8%.00619%7%.05 The Results… What conclusion should we draw about the effect of bupropion in the standard counseling group?

Authors’ conclusions/Media coverage… “Among the women who received standard counseling, bupropion did not appear to improve quit rates or time to relapse.” “For the women who received standard counseling, taking bupropion didn't seem to make a difference.”

Correct take-home message… Bupropion improves quitting rates over counseling alone. Main effect for drug is significant. Main effect for counseling type is NOT significant. Interaction between drug and counseling type is NOT significant.

Pitfall 5: the fallacy of comparing statistical significance “the effect was significant in the treatment group, but not significant in the control group” does not imply that the groups differ significantly

Example In a placebo-controlled randomized trial of DHA oil for eczema, researchers found a statistically significant improvement in the DHA group but not the placebo group. The abstract reports: “DHA, but not the control treatment, resulted in a significant clinical improvement of atopic eczema.” However, the improvement in the treatment group was not significantly better than the improvement in the placebo group, so this is actually a null result.

Misleading “significance comparisons” Koch C, Dölle S, Metzger M, et al. Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) supplementation in atopic eczema: a randomized, double-blind, controlled trial. Br J Dermatol 2008;158: The improvement in the DHA group (18%) is not significantly greater than the improvement in the control group (11%).

Within-group vs. between-group tests Examples of statistical tests used to evaluate within-group effects versus statistical tests used to evaluate between-group effects Statistical tests for within-group effectsStatistical tests for between-group effects Paired ttestTwo-sample ttest Wilcoxon sign-rank testWilcoxon sum-rank test (equivalently, Mann-Whitney U test) Repeated-measures ANOVA, time effectANOVA; repeated-measures ANOVA, group*time effect McNemar’s testDifference in proportions, Chi-square test, or relative risk

Also applies to interactions… Similarly, “we found a significant effect in subgroup 1 but not subgroup 2” does not constitute prove of interaction For example, if the effect of a drug is significant in men, but not in women, this is not proof of a drug-gender interaction.

Within-subgroup significance vs. interaction Rates of biochemically verified prolonged abstinence at 3, 6, and 12 months from a four-arm randomized trial of smoking cessation * Month s after quit target date Weight-focused counselingStandard counseling groupP-value for interaction between bupropion and counseling type ** Bupropion group abstinence (n=106) Placebo group abstinence (n=87) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo Bupropion group abstinence (n=89) Placebo group abstinence (n=67) P-value, bupropion vs. placebo 341%18%.00133%19% %11%.00121%10% %8%.00619%7% * From Tables 2 and 3: Levine MD, Perkins KS, Kalarchian MA, et al. Bupropion and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Weight-Concerned Women Smokers. Arch Intern Med 2010;170: ** Interaction p-values were newly calculated from logistic regression based on the abstinence rates and sample sizes shown in this table.

Statistical Power Statistical power is the probability of finding an effect if it’s real.

Can we quantify how much power we have for given sample sizes?

Null Distribution: difference=0. Clinically relevant alternative: difference=10%. Rejection region. Any value >= 6.5 (0+3.3*1.96) study 1: 263 cases, 1241 controls For 5% significance level, one-tail area=2.5% (Z  /2 = 1.96) Power= chance of being in the rejection region if the alternative is true=area to the right of this line (in yellow) 

Power here = >80% Rejection region. Any value >= 6.5 (0+3.3*1.96) Power= chance of being in the rejection region if the alternative is true=area to the right of this line (in yellow) study 1: 263 cases, 1241 controls

Critical value= 0+10*1.96=20 Power closer to 20% now. 2.5% area Z  /2 =1.96 study 1: 50 cases, 50 controls

Critical value= *1.96 = 1 Power is nearly 100%! Study 2: 18 treated, 72 controls, STD DEV = 2 Clinically relevant alternative: difference=4 points

Critical value= *1.96 = 5 Power is about 40% Study 2: 18 treated, 72 controls, STD DEV=10

Critical value= *1.96 = 1 Power is about 50% Study 2: 18 treated, 72 controls, effect size=1.0 Clinically relevant alternative: difference=1 point

Factors Affecting Power 1. Size of the effect 2. Standard deviation of the characteristic 3. Bigger sample size 4. Significance level desired

average weight from samples of 100 Null Clinically relevant alternative 1. Bigger difference from the null mean

average weight from samples of Bigger standard deviation

average weight from samples of Bigger Sample Size

average weight from samples of Higher significance level Rejection region.

Sample size calculations Based on these elements, you can write a formal mathematical equation that relates power, sample size, effect size, standard deviation, and significance level…

Simple formula for difference in proportions Sample size in each group (assumes equal sized groups) Represents the desired power (typically.84 for 80% power). Represents the desired level of statistical significance (typically 1.96). A measure of variability (similar to standard deviation) Effect Size (the difference in proportions)

Simple formula for difference in means Sample size in each group (assumes equal sized groups) Represents the desired power (typically.84 for 80% power). Represents the desired level of statistical significance (typically 1.96). Standard deviation of the outcome variable Effect Size (the difference in means)

Sample size calculators on the web… n/view/Main/PowerSampleSize n/view/Main/PowerSampleSize _size/size.html _size/size.html

These sample size calculations are idealized They do not account for losses-to-follow up (prospective studies) They do not account for non-compliance (for intervention trial or RCT) They assume that individuals are independent observations (not true in clustered designs) Consult a statistician!

Review Question 1 Which of the following elements does not increase statistical power? a. Increased sample size b. Measuring the outcome variable more precisely c. A significance level of.01 rather than.05 d. A larger effect size.

Review Question 2 Most sample size calculators ask you to input a value for . What are they asking for? a. The standard error b. The standard deviation c. The standard error of the difference d. The coefficient of deviation e. The variance

Review Question 3 For your RCT, you want 80% power to detect a reduction of 10 points or more in the treatment group relative to placebo. What is 10 in your sample size formula? a. Standard deviation b. mean change c. Effect size d. Standard error e. Significance level

Homework Problem Set 5 Reading: The Problem of Multiple Testing; Misleading Comparisons: The Fallacy of Comparing Statistical Significance (on Coursework) Reading: Chapters Vickers Journal article/article review sheet