I Need Out Because He Wants In the House: The Subject Pronoun in need and want Phrasal Constructions 1 Gregory Paules & Dr. Erica J. Benson English Department,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
SUBJECT VERB AGREEMENT Adventures in Grammar!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
Advertisements

The Northern Cities Shift in real- and apparent-time: Evidence from Chicago Corrine McCarthy George Mason University.
Acknowledgments We are grateful to the Family History Center in Eau Claire, Wisconsin for their aid in obtaining and utilizing London’s 1665 parish records.
“to be” singular I am You are He, she, it is plural We are You are They are 1st 2nd 3rd.
Attitudes toward Language Variation and the Liberal Arts 1 Anna Merry & Dr. Erica J. Benson  English  University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire Research Focus.
Introduction to phrases & clauses
Me First!: The Use of Personal Pronouns in Canadian English Katarina Fischer, Jenny Kenmir, Natalie Poirier Queen’s University Hypotheses 1.Native Canadian.
Nouns, Verbs, Adjectives, Adverbs.  Structure Classes: ◦ stable – changes little over time; about 1% of words in English; help put structure of sentences.
The Parts of the Sentence.  Every complete sentence must have at least one subject and one verb.  Although it is not necessary to have one in a sentence,
Gender Bias in the Mass Media: Evaluating Rebellion Feminisms, Political Correctness and “The Other” Caitlin Anderson and Samantha Michaelson Funding for.
Corpus Linguistics Case study 2 Grammatical studies based on morphemes or words. G Kennedy (1998) An introduction to corpus linguistics, London: Longman,
Phonetics, Phonology, Morphology and Syntax
Top Ten Tips for teachers preparing students for the academic version of IELTS Sam McCarter Macmillan Online Conference 2013.
Latin Grammar: Singular and Plural Magister Henderson Latin I.
Journal Article Presentation Group 1: Anik Damaris Maria Rofik.
PARTS OF SPEECH.
What are imperatives? Why do we care? The Solution: A brief syntactic background: Movement in X-bar theory: Paula Hagen  English Linguistics  University.
Introduction Mel- Frequency Cepstral Coefficients (MFCCs) are quantitative representations of speech and are commonly used to label sound files. They are.
What Do UWEC Students and Chippewa Valley Households Think About Global Warming? Students: Nicholas Kuqali, Ben Ponkratz, Matt Sackmann and Paul Stroik.
Infinitive Phrases Advanced Composition. Infinitives A verbal that functions as a noun, an adjective, or an adverb. An infinitive usually begins with.
Jelena Mirković and Maryellen C. MacDonald Language and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of Wisconsin-Madison Introduction How to Study Subject-Verb.
Grammar Appositive Phrases
Employment Effects Across Recessionary Periods for the US, Wisconsin, and the Chippewa Valley: UWEC Economics Students: Blake Barnes, Ross.
Background RateMyProfessors.com (RMP.com) is a public forum where students rate instructors on several characteristics: Clarity Helpfulness Overall Quality.
Paraphrasing and Plagiarism. PLAGIARISM Plagiarism is using data, ideas, or words that originated in work by another person without appropriately acknowledging.
By: Hawks.  18 Sentence error identification questions  25 sentence improvement questions  6 paragraph improvement questions.
A Longitudinal Study of Writing by Psychology Majors: A Quantitative Text Analysis A Longitudinal Study of Writing by Psychology Majors: A Quantitative.
Dr. Kenny. COPY THE FOLLOWING: It was (she, her) who came with us to the movies. (I, Me) gave into the pressure. All of us would rather be with (he, him)
Dr. Monira Al-Mohizea MORPHOLOGY & SYNTAX WEEK 12.
Language Learning Targets based on CLIMB standards.
What is Usability Testing? The NatureMapping Website 1 English Department, University of Wisconsin—Eau Claire  2 University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire 
The Impact of the Great Recession on Social Assistance Programs in the Eau Claire Area Students: Michael Dabat, Samantha Faber, Dan Hartson, Tony Navara,
Adele E. Goldberg. How argument structure constructions are learned.
Modeling the Effects of Stream Microhabitat on Group Size and Foraging Success of Juvenile Coho Salmon William Hintz, Matthew Faust, and Mitch Banach Dr.
Phrases and Clauses L/O: to revise/learn how to analyse larger units of language – phrases and clauses to revise/learn how to analyse larger units of language.
AN INTRO TO LINGUISTICS CREATED BY TENAYA CAMPBELL.
 I walk,  You walk,  He or she walks. (singular)  We walk  You walk  They walk. (plural)
An Investigation of World Language Teaching Methodologies and Language Usage in the Classroom Kaitlyn Hellenbrand (Dr. Anne Hlas) Department of Foreign.
Prepositions. What is a prepositional phrase? They tell: WHERE, WHEN AND HOW Begin with a preposition and end with a noun or pronoun Will not be the subject.
Regional Dialects Wolfram & Schilling-Estes Chapter 5.
The purpose of this research project was to find a correlation between concordance rates for communication disorders in monozygotic and dizygotic twins.
UNIT 8: CH. 24 AGREEMENT 7 th English. SUBJECT/VERB AGREEMENT 1. A verb must agree with its subject in number (singular or plural). -Nouns are made plural.
COPY THESE RULES AND COMPLETE THE EXERCISE AGREEMENT RULE 1 When a word refers to one person or thing, it is singular in number. When a word refers.
Passive Generalizations Li, Charles N. & Thompson, Sandra A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese - A Functional Reference Grammar. Los Angeles: University of California.
The Structure of Language Finding Patterns in the Noise Presented by Cliff Jones, M.A., Linguistics.
Introduction Method United States Belgium Discussion Katherine Quigley & Emily Prosser Faculty Mentors: Dr. Jennifer Muehlenkamp, Department of Psychology,
Basic Syntactic Structures of English CSCI-GA.2590 – Lecture 2B Ralph Grishman NYU.
Parts of Speech By: Miaya Nischelle Sample. NOUN A noun is a person place or thing.
Lexico-grammatical means of expressing modality 1. What is modality? 2. Possibilities to express modality 3. Intrinsic vs. Extrinsic 4. References.
Inflection. Inflection refers to word formation that does not change category and does not create new lexemes, but rather changes the form of lexemes.
Prescriptive grammar. Prescriptive Grammar: a set of prescribed rules which tells people how to speak/write 18 century.
Use of Concordancers A corpus (plural corpora) – a large collection of texts, written or spoken, stored on a computer. A concordancer – a computer programme.
1 Variation in English Grammar Linda Thomas U210A Chapter 6.
Using language corpora in developing Arabic lessons & syllabuses
Introduction to Linguistics
Subject Pronouns.
Noella Handley M.A. Student in Linguistics
Making sense of phrasal verbs with “look”
Prepositions.
Exploring Consumer’s Reaction in the Bad Gift Situation Across Cultures Student Researchers: Gracia Clark and Yooik Jo | Department of Psychology, University.
Prepositional Phrases as Adjectives and Adverbs
HOW TO TRANSLATE FROM LATIN INTO ENGLISH!!!
Sample of Tagger Accuracy Testing
I can identify and use SUBJECT PRONOUNS.
Past and Present: Verb Tenses Across Blog Topics
ENGLISH LANGUAGE CLAIRE WALLIS
Prepositions and Prepositional Phrases
Parts of Speech II.
Adverbs and adverbial What about "Wendy could see a house at the end of the street“? What is ‘at the end of the street? This sentence is ambiguous. First.
8 Parts of speech Get your grammar on..
Presentation transcript:

I Need Out Because He Wants In the House: The Subject Pronoun in need and want Phrasal Constructions 1 Gregory Paules & Dr. Erica J. Benson English Department, University of Wisconsin – Eau Claire Introduction: In some dialects of American English, the following sentences are acceptable: 1.The dog wants in. (want + prepositional adverb) 2.He wants off the horse. (want + prepositional phrase) 3.I need out. (need + prepositional adverb) 4.She needs on the bus. (need + prepositional phrase) Other dialects of American English require an infinitive verb between the verbs want and need and the prepositional adverb or prepositional phrase (e.g., I need to go out.) Studies on the geographic distribution of want + prepositional adverb/phrase and need + prepositional adverb/phrase have found these constructions to be commonly used in the Midland dialect area (see Fig. 1) and recent work suggests that these constructions are spreading to other dialect areas (e.g., Kurath 1949; DARE; Benson 2009 & in progress). The disparity in usage patterns within the Midland dialect area and in the spread of various forms of these constructions to other areas suggest that other linguistic factors, in particular, syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic factors may be playing a role in their distribution. Figure 1. General Dialect Area Map of the United States (Labov et al. 2006) Research Objective: In this project, which is part of a larger project examining syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic factors affecting want + prepositional adverb/phrase and need + prepositional adverb/phrase, we explore the role that one syntactic factor, namely subject type, plays in the widespread distribution of these constructions in written sources. More specifically, we investigate the frequency of different types of subjects and subject pronouns (e.g., I, he) and the role they play in the acceptability of the four broad types of these constructions: want + prepositional adverb (as in 1), want + prepositional phrase (as in 2), need + prepositional adverb (as in 3), need + prepositional phrase (as in 4). Methods: From July-December 2008, we compiled a corpus of examples of want + prepositional adverb/phrase and need + prepositional adverb/phrase by doing an extensive search of American English sources on-line, including print media, books, websites, and open blogs. 2 Our corpus consists of 247 examples of want + prepositional adverb, 276 examples of want + prepositional phrase, 66 examples of need + prepositional adverb, and 103 examples of need + prepositional phrase. The data in our corpus are compared to the frequencies found in the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA), which we use as a baseline for "General American Usage." Subject Type: Want Constructions compared to General American Usage (COCA) Subject Type: Need Constructions compared to General American Usage (COCA) Discussion & Conclusions: Overall, want + prepositional adverb and want + prepositional phrase show similar distribution patterns by subject type as general American, with slight increases in want + prepositional phrase in the 1 st person and want + prepositional adverb with 3 rd person pronouns. More striking, however, is that in need + prepositional adverb and need + prepositional phrase, 1 st person subjects (I, we) are favored over other subject types. Perhaps most notable is that all four types of constructions strongly favor singular pronoun subjects over plural pronoun subjects (in contrast with general American usage). As a result of these differences in distribution by subject types, we might expect to find: (1) greater acceptability of want + prepositional phrase, need + prepositional adverb, and need + prepositional phrase with 1 st person subjects (rather than 2 nd person (pronoun) subjects (i.e., you) and 3 rd person ) and (2) greater acceptability of all four constructions with singular subjects (rather than plural subjects) in all dialect areas and especially in those to which these need/want constructions are spreading. Singular vs. Plural Subject: Want Constructions compared to General American Usage (COCA) Singular vs. Plural Subject: Need Constructions compared to General American Usage (COCA) In our corpus, the want + prepositional phrase construction shows a higher percentage of 1 st person (pronoun) subjects (i.e., I, we) than in the want + prepositional adverb construction and in general American usage of 1 st person subjects with want; see Fig. 2. Overall, 2 nd person (pronoun) subjects (i.e., you) with want were less common than with other subject/pronoun types; however, this may be due to our focus on written language. Of all subject types, 3 rd person subjects are the most common of all subject types and among those, nouns are more common than pronouns (i.e., he, she, it, they); see Fig. 2. Interestingly, the want + prepositional adverb construction shows a remarkably higher percentage of 3 rd person pronoun subjects than the want + prepositional phrase constructions and the general American usage of 3 rd person pronoun subject with want; see Fig. 2. Most remarkable in Figure 3, is the switch from a higher percentage of 3 st person subjects (especially noun subjects)—both in general American usage and as compared with the want constructions in Figure 2—to 1 st person (pronoun) subjects (i.e., I, we) of need + prepositional adverb and need + prepositional phrase. The 2 nd person (pronoun) subjects (i.e., you) and 3 rd person pronoun subjects (i.e., he, she, it, they) in need + prepositional adverb and need + prepositional phrase constructions are essentially equal with general American usage (COCA); see Fig. 3. With 1 st and 3 rd person pronoun subjects, the singular forms (I & he, she, it) are strongly favored over the plural forms (we & they) in the want constructions far exceeding the general American usage of singular pronoun subjects in the 1 st and 3 rd person in COCA; see Fig. 4. It was impossible to distinguish 2 nd person singular and plural pronoun subjects (I.e., you) in the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA), so we haven't reported on those. Like the want constructions (in Fig. 4), the need constructions, shown in Figure 5, favor singular pronoun subjects in the 1 st and 3 rd person (I & he, she, it) over the plural pronoun subjects (we & they) in stark contrast to general American usage in COCA. Selected References: Benson, Erica J Everyone wants in: Want + prepositional adverb in the Midland and Beyond. Journal of English Linguistics 37.1: Davies, Mark. Corpus of contemporary American English. Brigham Young University. Kurath, Hans A word geography of the eastern United States. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Labov, William, Sharon Ash & Charles Boberg Atlas of North American English. Berlin: Mouton. Figure 2. Subject type in want + prepositional adverb and want + prepositional phrase compared to General American Usage (COCA) Figure 4. Singular and plural subjects in want + prepositional adverb and want + prepositional phrase compared to General American Usage (COCA) Figure 3. Subject type in need + prepositional adverb and need + prepositional phrase compared to General American Usage (COCA) Figure 5. Singular and plural subjects in need + prepositional adverb and need + prepositional phrase compared to General American Usage (COCA) Contact Information: or Footnotes: 1 We gratefully acknowledge the resources provided by the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs in the form of a Faculty/Student Research Collaboration Differential Tuition Grant, the English Department, as well as the printing resources provided by Learning and Technology Services and the University of Wisconsin–Eau Claire’s Differential Tuition program. 2 We owe a debt of gratitude to Michael J. Fox, Michelle Hazard, Graeme Lewis, and Heidi Kraemer for their help gathering examples for our corpus.