O Children exposed to higher levels of interparental conflict appear to be more sensitive to later parental disagreements o They report greater threat,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Family and peer factors related to the development of aggression in adolescent dating relationships John H. Grych & Kara Lindstedt Marquette University.
Advertisements

Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
Attributions of Fathering Behaviors Among Adolescents: The Role of Depressive Symptoms, Ethnicity, and Family Structure Andrea K. Finlay 1, Jeffrey T.
The Mediating Role of Forgiveness in Mental Health and Adjustment The Mediating Role of Forgiveness in Mental Health and Adjustment Kimberly M. Jorgensen,
Domestic Violence, Parenting, and Behavior Outcomes of Children Chien-Chung Huang Rutgers University.
Marital Satisfaction and the Development of Autonomy and Close Friendships in Early Adolescence Jessica R. Meyer L. Wrenn Thompson Kathleen Boykin McElhaney.
Aggression: Behavior aimed at harming or injuring others – May be Physical: Harms others through physical injury (exs: pushing, hitting, destroying another’s.
Abstract Investigated the mother-child relationship and adolescents’ emotional symptoms and adaptive skills reported by youth. Negative mother-child relationship.
The Comparison of Friendships of Adult Children of Divorce and Adult Children of Intact Families Meghan M. Tweed Jennifer L. Crum Hanover College.
Emotion and Relationship Effects on Gender Differences in Empathy Method Participants were 60 women and 24 men. Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis,
Attachment – Lesson Three
What Makes the Finger Point Internally? Predictors of Self-Blame/Guilt in Sexually Abused Boys and Girls ????? ???????? University of Nebraska-Lincoln.
Interparental Conflict, Young Adult Adjustment, and Appraisals Among Emerging Adults Christine R. Keeports & Laura D. Pittman Northern Illinois University.
 Past research examining the relationship between interparental conflict (IPC) and children’s adjustment has emphasized the importance of emotional, cognitive,
Interparental Conflict & Children’s Internalizing Psychopathology: Examining the Role of Children’s Appraisals & Emotions Jennifer K. Hauser & John H.
Emotional Processes Involved in Children's Adjustment to Interparental Conflict: An Attachment Perspective Sarah Raynor, Stacy Braun, and John Grych Marquette.
Spat, Spite and Spit: Interparental Conflict, Child Adjustment and the Role of Children’s Physiological Stress-System Activity Patricia Pendry Washington.
Predicting and Avoiding Conflict & Nonviolent Conflict Resolution.
EMOTIONAL AND SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT IN MIDDLE CHILDHOOD
Parenting Primary School Age Children Dr Louise Keown Faculty of Education.
SPSS Session 4: Association and Prediction Using Correlation and Regression.
WHAT DO CHILDREN DO DURING INCIDENTS OF DOMESTIC VIOLENCE.
Middle School Predictors of High School Dating Violence Among At-Risk Early Adolescents Meredith C. Joppa, Ph.D. 1, Christopher D. Houck, Ph.D. 2, and.
THE IMPACT OF COMPUTER SELF-EFFICACY AND TECHNOLOGY DEPENDENCE ON COMPUTER-RELATED TECHNOSTRESS: A SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY PERSPECTIVE Qin Shu, Qiang Tu.
Part I begins: Components of Conflict Chapter 1: Perspectives on Conflict.
By;David Salinas Adolescent communication. Introduction An adolescent is considered to be the teen years between the ages of This long period is.
Katherine Little, F. Christy McFarland, Deborah Land, Jennifer Haynes, & Joseph Allen Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research.
Adolescents in High Risk Families: Are There Different Dynamics at Home That Affect Behavior Problems? Karina R. Sokol, Priscila Diaz, William Fabricius,
 Triangulation into Interparental Conflict  The act of involving adolescents in conflicts between parents  This may include one parent enlisting the.
The impact of attachment and depression symptoms on multiple risk behaviors in post-war adolescents in northern Uganda Okello J, Nakimuli-Mpungu E, Klasen.
Ethnic Identity among Mexican American Adolescents: The Role of Maternal Cultural Values and Parenting Practices 1 Miriam M. Martinez, 1 Gustavo Carlo,
Maternal Romantic Relationship Quality, Parenting Stress and Child Outcomes: A Mediational Model Christine R. Keeports, Nicole J. Holmberg, & Laura D.
Introduction Family systems theory provides a theoretically rich foundation from which to investigate family processes; however, little empirical work.
Describing and predicting changes in interparental conflict across early adolescence: A latent curve model analysis Abstract Discussion Tables and Figures.
Experiences with Parents, Peers, and Romantic Partners During Adolescence as Predictors of Youths’ Emotion Regulation Strategies David E. Szwedo, M.A.
Seminar on Theories in Child Development: Overview Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos.
Socioemotional Development Infancy and Early Childhood Chapter 5.
Early Adolescent Behaviors in Disagreement with Best Friend Predictive of Later Emotional Repair Abilities Lauren Cannavo, Elenda T. Hessel, Joseph S.
Temperament Constitutionally based individual differences in behavioral characteristics that are relatively consistent across situations and over time.
Gender differences in symptom reporting: the influence of psychological traits. Laura Goodwin Dr Stephen Fairclough Liverpool John Moores University BACKGROUND.
Youth violence exposure, adolescent delinquency and anxiety, and the potential mediating role of sleep problems during middle childhood Chelsea M. Weaver.
Attachment and Development in Adolescent Romantic Couples’ Relationship Quality Sharon C. Risch University of Tennessee.
The Reliability of Interparental and Peer Reports on Adolescent Delinquency Martin Ho and Farah Williams University of Virginia Acknowledgements: We gratefully.
Adolescent attachment state-of-mind and parent-adolescent relationship quality: Joint longitudinal predictors of social and emotional functioning Kathleen.
Adolescent Peer and Romantic Predictors of Youths’ Emotion Regulation in Early Adulthood Introduction David E. Szwedo David E. Szwedo,
Early Shyness and School-Age Internalizing Symptoms: Mediation by Emotion Regulation Xin Feng 1, Daniel S. Shaw 2, and Kristin L. Moilanen 3 1 The Ohio.
Outcome(s)  Assess the extent to which biological, cognitive, and sociocultural factors influence development  Evaluate psychological research relevant.
Explanations Cognitive Psychodynamic Treatments Cognitive psychodynamic.
Mothering Profiles Observed in Low-Income Ethnic Minority Families: Relations to Maternal Depression and Family Risk Factors Nazly Dyer*, Margaret Tresch.
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
Longitudinal Effects of Mothers’ Encouragement and Discouragement of Positive Emotions on Children’s Task Behavior Deon Brown, Julie C. Dunsmore, Erika.
Florida International University, Miami, FL
DISCUSSION (continues) ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS AND CONTACT
Kreila Cote, Christie Miksys, Sapir Sasson, Jennifer Milliken, Julie L
Parental Affective Reactions to Prolonged Infant Crying: Does Risk Status or Parent Gender Matter? Kreila Cote, Christie Miksys, Sapir Sasson, Jennifer.
Inter-Parent Aggression Predicts Children’s Relationally but not Physically Aggressive Social Problem Solving Amanda W. Harrist1, Julie M. Rutledge2,
Introduction Hypotheses Results Discussion Method
Social Cognition Aggression
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Predicting and Avoiding Conflict & Nonviolent Conflict Resolution
Introduction Discussion Results Method References
In pairs complete the Agony Aunt task
Introduction Results Conclusions Method
Introduction Results Conclusions Method
Introduction Results Conclusions Hypotheses Method
General Social Competence (18)
Korey F. Beckwith & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
The Effects of Childhood Emotional Abuse on Later Romantic Relationship Outcomes: The Moderating Role of Self-Worth, Alcohol, and Jealousy Madeline M.
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Health and Wellbeing Understanding Behaviour and Calming Ideas
Presentation transcript:

o Children exposed to higher levels of interparental conflict appear to be more sensitive to later parental disagreements o They report greater threat, negative affect, and physiological reactivity (e.g., El-Sheikh, et al., 1994; Grych, 1998). o The mechanism underlying this sensitization effect is not clear, but may involve expectations about the course of parental conflicts that children develop from earlier experiences with interparental conflict (e.g., Fosco, DeBoard, & Grych, 2007). o Children who have witnessed intense, poorly resolved disagreements may anticipate that conflict will escalate o Children whose parents typically resolve conflict constructively may expect disagreements to be mild and short-lived. o Children who experience greater threat and self-blame in response to parental discord also may be sensitive to signs of anger and conflict and more likely to anticipate that it will escalate. o There also be an affective basis for sensitization to conflict o Children who are highly distressed by conflict may have more difficulty regulating their affect when parental disagreements occur (Davies & Cummings, 1994). o There may be gender differences in children’s appraisals of interparental conflict o Previous research has shown that girls tend to focus more on the affect expressed during interparental conflict than boys do to help them assess the threat posed by the conflict (Grych, 1998). The goal of this study was to examine whether children’s prior exposure to interparental conflict, appraisals of interparental conflict, and capacity for emotion regulation predict how they respond to a new parental disagreement. Exploring Cognitive and Affective Processes that Underlie Children’s Sensitization to Interparental Conflict Anna J. Kiel and John H. Grych Funding Provided By NIMH# MH Results MethodIntroductionDiscussion Study Goal Sample Participants were year olds from diverse ethnic backgrounds (47% female) Conflict Interaction Task Children viewed a videotaped interaction in which two actors portrayed a parental disagreement. The interaction was shown in 3 segments: (1) the parents begin to discuss an issue and initial signs of disagreement occur (2) the interaction becomes more conflictual and angry, ending with the father walking out of the room (3) the father returns, apologizes to the mother, and the video ends without the viewer knowing if they will resolve the disagreement Coding Children’s Expectation of Conflict Interaction At the end of each video segment, children were asked to describe what they thought would happen next. We coded their expectation for the course of the disagreement as resolving, continuing, or escalating (See Table 1) Assessing Interparental Conflict Children completed the CPIC (Grych et al., 1992) to assess their prior exposure to conflict and their appraisals of threat and self- blame. Parents completed the CPS (Kerig, 1995) to assess interparental conflict in the home. Table 1 Examples of Codes for Outcome Expectations of Videotaped Interaction _________________________________________________________ CodesExamples Resolve“I think next the mom and dad will talk through it and come to an agreement.” “Next, they are going to agree and be done talking about it.” Continue“I think the mom and dad are going to keep arguing like they’re doing now.” “The mom and dad won’t agree on what to do, so they will keep arguing about it.” Escalate“I think the mom and dad are going to start a big fight and they will get divorced.” “The dad is going to throw something and the mom will call the cops.” Emotion Regulation Parents reported on children’s emotion regulation with the ERC (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997). Table 2 Frequencies and Percentages of Girls’ and Boys’ Outcome Expectations After the Three Video Segments ____________________________________________________________________________________ Expectation Segment 1 Segment 2 Segment 3 GirlsBoysGirlsBoysGirlsBoys Resolve3 (5.4%)5 (7.9%)23 (41.4%)27 (42.9%)50 (89.3%)57 (90.5%) Continue50 (89.3%)52 (82.5%)14 (25.0%)18 (28.6%)0 (0.0%)0 (0.0%) Escalate1 (1.8%)4 (6.3%)11 (19.6%)14 (22.2%)0 (0.0%)0 (0.0%) Missing2 (3.6%)2 (3.2%)8 (14.3%)4 (6.3%)6 (10.7%)6 (9.5%) Table 3 Pearson Correlations Between Children’s Expectations After Video Segments 1 and 2, Children’s Ratings of Perceived Threat, Self-Blame, Conflict Properties, Parents’ Ratings of Conflict at Home, and Parents’ Ratings of Emotion Regulation (Girls Above Diagonal, Boys Below Diagonal) _______________________________________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________________________________ 1. Expectation after Segment 1 _ * * 2. Expectation after Segment 2.269*_ Child Rating of Perceived Threat _.329*.609** Child Rating of Self-Blame _.391**.433**.447** 5. Child Rating of Conflict Properties **.369**_.467** Parents’ Rating of Conflict at Home *.564**_.488** 7. Parents’ Rating of Emotion Regulation * _ * p <.05 (2-tailed), **p <.01 (2-tailed) Patterns of associations among variables differed for girls and boys (see Tables 2 and 3). Girls After viewing the first video segment, girls who engaged in more self-blame expected the conflict video to have a better resolution (r = -.33, p <.05). After viewing the first video segment, girls who were rated as having poorer emotion regulation by their parents expected the conflict video to have a better resolution (r = -.33, p <.05). Girls who engaged in more self-blame reported more conflict properties (r =.391, p <.01), their parents reported more interparental conflict (r =.433, p <.01), and they were rated by their parents as having poorer emotion regulation (r =.433, p <.01). Boys After viewing the second video segment, boys who were rated as having poorer emotion regulation by their parents expected the conflict video to have a poorer resolution (r =.315, p <.05). Boys who perceived more threat at home reported more conflict properties at home (r =.363, p <.01). Boys who engaged in more self-blame reported that there were more conflict properties at home (r =.369, p <.01), and their parents reported more interparental conflict (r =.315, p <.05). Selected References Davies, P. & Cummings, E. (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, El-Sheikh, M. (1994). Children’s emotional and physiological responses to interadult angry behavior: The role of history of interparental hostility. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, Fosco, G., Deboard, R. & Grych, J. (2007). Making sense of family violence: Implications of children’s appraisals of interparental aggression for their short- and long-term functioning. European Psychologist, 12, Grych, J.H. (1998). Children’s appraisals of interparental conflict: Situational and contextual influences. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, Grych, J.H., Seid, M., & Fincham, F.D. (1992). Assessing Marital Conflict from the Child's Perspective: The Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Child Development, 63(3), 558. Kerig, P. K. (1995, March). Assessing the links between marital conflict and child development. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN. Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), Girls Girls in this study who engaged in more self-blame experienced more conflict in their homes and were rated as having poorer emotion regulation. It may be that they were used to more intense conflict between parents than they were exposed to in this study. Because the conflict between the parent actors on the video was fairly mild, it may not be eliciting the same kind of expectations about interparental conflict that these girls normally have. This could explain why the girls in this study who engage in more self- blame expected the video conflict to have a better resolution – they did not perceive the disagreement as intense, therefore they did not think it would end poorly. Findings of this study provide further evidence that girls who are exposed to higher levels of conflict in the home have more difficulty regulating their emotions. Boys Boys with poorer emotion regulation in this study expected the video conflict to have a poorer outcome after viewing the most intense segment of the video (segment 2). This means that these boys were more sensitive to the conflict in the video than were the girls with poor emotion regulation, and boys responded to the conflict by expecting it to resolve poorly. It is likely that boys’ prior exposure to interparental conflict was driving their expectations about the outcome of the video conflict. Boys who experienced more conflict at home engaged in more self-blame and were rated as having poorer emotion regulation than boys who experienced less conflict, which supports previous research findings that interparental conflict has negative effects on children’s emotion regulation abilities. Selected References