Chapter 3 Learning and Memory

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Learning & Consumer Behavior
Advertisements

Consumer Learning.
Class 6 Learning and Memory CA 2018 Consumer Insight A.Kwanta Sirivajjanangkul A.Panitta Kanchanavasita Albert Laurence School of Communication Arts Department.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
LEARNING, MEMORY, AND PRODUCT POSITIONING
Chapter 6: Learning. Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov A type of learning in which a neutral stimulus acquires the ability to elicit a response. How.
PSYCHOLOGY: LEARNING Learning- the process of acquiring new and relatively enduring information or behaviors.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Introduction to Psychology, 7th Edition, Rod Plotnik Module 9: Classical Conditioning Module 9 Classical Conditioning.
Learning and Memory. What is Learning? A change in Behaviour caused by experience. What is Consumer learning?
Learning and Memory. What is Learning? A change in Behaviour caused by experience.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Consumer Behavior BA 492 Winter 2007 Learning and Memory.
Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Presented by: Heather Hays.
INFORMATION PROCESSING: Learning & Memory Learning Learning is any change in the content or organization of long-term memory or behavior caused by experience.
Learning and Memory.
Learning and Memory. What is Learning? A change in Behaviour caused by experience. What is Consumer learning?
Learning and Memory Chapter 3.
Learning and Memory. LearningLearning refers to a relatively permanent change in behaviour that is caused by experience. It’s an ongoing process. »Vicarious.
Learning and Memory. What is Learning? A change in Behaviour caused by experience. What is Consumer learning?
3-1 Chapter 3 Learning and Memory. 3-2 The Learning Process Learning refers to a relatively permanent change in behavior that is caused by experience.
Chapter 6 Consumer Learning
Learning and Memory Michael Kalsher MGMT 4460/6962 Summer 2014.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Consumer Behavior, Eighth Edition SCHIFFMAN & KANUK
PEMBELAJARAN KONSUMEN Pertemuan 06
1 MKT201 - Weeks 3 & 4 Learning and Memory (Ch. 3)
Learning and Memory.
Instructions for using this template. Remember this is Jeopardy, so where I have written “Answer” this is the prompt the students will see, and where.
Motivating Learners.
Chapter 6: Learning. Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov Terminology –Unconditioned Stimulus (UCS): evokes an unconditioned response without previous conditioning.
INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY
Learning Theories Learning To gain knowledge, understanding, or skill, by study, instruction, or experience.
4 th Edition Copyright 2004 Prentice Hall5-1 Learning Chapter 5.
Review Unit 7. Observational Learning Learning by watching others.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Learning and Memory 3-1 CONSUMER BEHAVIOR, 10e Michael R. Solomon.
© 2009 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. CHAPTER 7 Behavioral and Social Cognitive Approaches.
5 ConsumerLearning 5 Consumer Learning. Learning Is a Key to Consumer Behavior 9-1 Culture Subcultures Social class Family Friends Institutions Personal.
CHAPTER NINE Learning, Memory, and Product Positioning McGraw-Hill/Irwin Copyright © 2004 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.
Dr. M. Davis-Brantley.  Learning is the process that produces a relatively enduring change in behavior or knowledge as a result of an individual’s past.
Chapter 9 Learning.
1 MKT201 – Buyer Behavior Chapter 3 Review Questions.
Ch. 9: Learning and Memory Learning – any change in the content or organization of long-term memory and/or behavior. Consumer Learning - A process by which.
With Duane Weaver.  Learning Defined  Behavioural Learning  Cognitive Learning  Memory DefinedBehaviouralCognitiveMemory.
Chapter 7 Consumer Learning.  Marketers must teach consumers:  where to buy  how to use  how to maintain  how to dispose of products Importance of.
Learning Psychology. Bell Activity 3/22/2013 Learning Targets: At the end of class you will be able to- -Define Classical Conditioning -Define and Identify.
3 - 1 Chapter 3 Learning and Memory By Michael R. Solomon Consumer Behavior Buying, Having, and Being Sixth Edition.
Chapter 5 Learning. Copyright © 1999 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. 2 Defining Learning Learning –a relatively permanent change in behavior that occurs.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. THREE KINDS OF LEARNING Learning –A relatively enduring or permanent change in behavior that results from previous experience.
Chapter 7 Consumer Learning. ©2000 Prentice Hall Cognitive Learning Theory A theory of learning based on mental information processing, often in response.
Learning. n The process by which experience leads to changes in knowledge, attitudes, and/or behavior. n Learning is relatively permanent. n Learning.
Chapter 7 Consumer Learning.
Chapter 7 Consumer Learning.
Consumer Learning CHAPTER SEVEN. A Simple Model of Consumer Decision Making Chapter One Slide2 Copyright 2010 Pearson Education, Inc.
Classical Conditioning. I. Pavlov Most famous example of classical conditioning Salivating dog What happened? When a stimulus that does not initially.
Consumer Learning CHAPTER SEVEN. Learning Objectives 1.To Understand the Process and Four Elements of Consumer Learning. 2.To Study Behavioral Learning.
Chapter 7 Consumer Learning
Chapter 6 Notes AP Tips. Know about classical conditioning and Ivan Pavlov. Classical conditioning: the repeated pairing of an unconditioned stimulus.
CHAPTER NINE Learning, Memory, and Product Positioning McGraw-Hill/Irwin Copyright © 2004 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.
Learning and Memory Chapter 3 Copyright 2008 Pearson Education Canada.
CHAPTER SEVEN Consumer Learning.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Perspective on Consumer Behavior Chapter 4
Learning and Memory.
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Chapter 3 Learning and Memory
Presentation transcript:

Chapter 3 Learning and Memory CONSUMER BEHAVIOR, 9e Michael R. Solomon 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Learning Objectives When you finish this chapter, you should understand why: It’s important for marketers to understand how consumers learn about products and services. Conditioning results in learning. Learned associations can generalize to other things and why this is important to marketers. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Learning Objectives (continued) When you finish this chapter, you should understand why: There is a difference between classical and instrumental conditioning. We learn by observing others’ behavior. Memory systems work. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Learning Objectives (continued) When you finish this chapter, you should understand why: The other products we associate with an individual product influences how we will remember it. Products help us to retrieve memories from our past. Marketers measure our memories about products and ads. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

The Learning Process Learning: a relatively permanent change in behavior caused by experience Incidental learning: casual, unintentional acquisition of knowledge Marketers realize that long-standing, learned connections between products and memories are a potent way to build and keep brand loyalty. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior. Sometimes we learn when we don’t even try. This is called incidental learning. Learning is an ongoing process. Psychologists who study learning use several theories to explain the learning process. We’ll cover these in this chapter. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Behavioral Learning Theories Behavioral learning theories: assume that learning takes place as the result of responses to external events. The behavioral learning theories assume that the consumer is like a black box. The black box indicates that we really don’t understand what happens in someone’s mind. The slide shows Figure 3.1 which depicts this view. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Types of Behavioral Learning Theories Classical conditioning: a stimulus that elicits a response is paired with another stimulus that initially does not elicit a response on its own. Instrumental conditioning (also, operant conditioning): the individual learns to perform behaviors that produce positive outcomes and to avoid those that yield negative outcomes. There are two theories which are types of behavioral learning theories: classical conditioning and instrumental conditioning. Instrumental conditioning is also called operant conditioning. It means to condition behavior using consequences. It refers to voluntary behaviors, while classical conditioning works to condition responses to involuntary behaviors. We’ll cover more about classical conditioning on the next slide. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov rang bell and put meat powder into dogs’ mouths; repeated until dogs salivated when the bell rang Meat powder = UCS (natural reaction is drooling) Bell = CS (dogs learned to drool when bell rang) Drooling = CR Classical conditioning occurs when a stimulus that elicits a response is paired with another stimulus that initially does not elicit a response on its own. Over time, the second stimulus causes a similar response because we associate it with the first stimulus. Ivan Pavlov, a Russian physiologist who conducted research on digestion in animals, first demonstrated this phenomenon in dogs. He paired a neutral stimulus (a bell) with a stimulus known to cause a salivation response in dogs. The powder was an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) because it was naturally capable of causing the response. Over time, the bell became a conditioned stimulus (CS). The bell did not initially cause salivation but the dogs learned to associate the bell with the meat powder and began to salivate at the sound of the bell only. The drooling of these canine consumers because of a sound was a conditioned response (CR). 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Marketing Applications of Repetition Repetition increases learning More exposures = increased brand awareness When exposure decreases, extinction occurs However, too MUCH exposure leads to advertising wear out Example: Izod crocodile on clothes Conditioning effects are more likely to occur after the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) have been paired several times. Repeated exposures to the association increase the strength of the associations and prevent decay of these associations in memory. Many classic advertising campaigns consist of product slogans repeated often to enhance recall. The Rolaids campaign which asked, ‘How do you spell relief? ROLAIDS’ is a personal favorite. But for this to work, the UCS must repeatedly be paired with the CS. Otherwise, extinction occurs. Extinction means that the association is forgotten. Even when associations are established, too much exposure can turn negative. In that case, the association may change in terms of whether it is perceived as positive or negative. That’s what happened to Izod when its logo became too exposed on a variety of clothing and products. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Marketing Applications of Stimulus Generalization Stimulus generalization: tendency for stimuli similar to a conditioned stimulus to evoke similar, unconditioned responses. Family branding Product line extensions Licensing Look-alike packaging The process of stimulus generalization is critical to branding and packaging decisions that try to capitalize on consumer’s positive associations with an existing brand or company name. Marketers can base some strategies on stimulus generalization. Family branding enables products to capitalize on the reputation of a company name. Marketers can use product line extensions by adding related products to an established brand. Licensing allows companies to rent well-known names. Distinctive packaging designs create strong associations with a particular brand. Companies that make generic or private-level brands and want to communicate a quality image often exploit this linkage when they put their products in similar packages to those of popular brands. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Discussion Some advertisers use well-known songs to promote their products. They often pay more for the song than for original compositions. How do you react when one of your favorite songs turns up in a commercial? Why do advertisers do this? How does this relate to learning theory? If you worked for an ad agency, how would you select songs for your clients? 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Instrumental Conditioning Behaviors = positive outcomes or negative outcomes Instrumental conditions occurs in one of these ways: Positive reinforcement Negative reinforcement Punishment Extinction Instrumental conditioning (or operant conditioning) occurs when we learn to perform behaviors that produce positive outcomes and avoid those that yield negative outcomes. Whereas responses in classical conditioning are involuntary and fairly simple, we make those in instrumental conditioning deliberately to obtain a goal. We may learn the desired behavior over a period of time as a shaping process rewards our intermediate actions. Instrumental conditioning occurs in one of three ways: 1) positive reinforcement, 2) negative reinforcement, and 3) punishment. Positive reinforcement comes in the form of a reward. Negative reinforcement shows how a negative outcome can be avoided. Punishment occurs when unpleasant events follow a response. Extinction occurs when there is no reinforcement. In other words, the conditioning is not activated because it is not reinforced. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Figure 3.2 Instrumental Conditioning 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Reinforcement Schedules in Instrumental Conditioning Fixed-interval (seasonal sales) Variable-interval (secret shoppers) Fixed-ratio (grocery-shopping receipt programs) Variable-ratio (slot machines) When fixed-interval reinforcement is used, the first response made brings the reward. When using variable-interval reinforcement, the time that must pass before one gets reinforced varies based on some average. Because you don’t know exactly when to expect the reinforcement, you have to respond at a consistent rate. For fixed-ratio reinforcement, reinforcement occurs only after a fixed number of responses. The schedule motivates you to continue performing the same behavior over and over. In variable-ratio reinforcement, one is reinforced after a certain number of responses but you don’t know how many responses are required. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Cognitive Learning Theories: Observational Learning We watch others; we model behavior Conditions for modeling to occur: The consumer’s attention must be directed to the appropriate model The consumer must remember what the model does and says The consumer must convert information to action The consumer must be motivated to perform actions Cognitive learning theory approaches stress the importance of internal mental processes. This perspective views people as problem solvers who actively use information from the world around them to master their environments. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Figure 3.3 The Observational Learning Process Modeling: imitating others’ behavior 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Role of Memory in Learning Memory: acquiring information and storing it over time so that it will be available when needed. Information-processing approach; Figure 3.4 Mind = computer and data = input/output Contemporary approaches to the study of memory employ an information-processing approach. They assume that the mind is in some ways like a computer. Data are input, processed and output for later use in revised form. The figure summarizes the memory process. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

How Information Gets Encoded Encode: mentally program meaning Types of meaning: Sensory meaning, such as the literal color or shape of a package Semantic meaning: symbolic associations Episodic memories: relate to events that are personally relevant Narrative: memories store information we acquire in story form The way we encode, or mentally program, information helps to determine how we will represent it in memory. In general, it’s more likely we’ll retain incoming data when we associate it with other information already in memory. A narrative or story holds a lot of the social information we acquire. Narratives tend to persuade people to construct mental pictures of the information they have seen or heard and this helps the memory to be strong. Sometimes we process a stimulus simply in terms of its sensory meaning, such as the literal color or shape of a package. We may experience a feeling of familiarity when we see an ad for a new snack foods we have recently tasted. In other cases, we encode meanings at a more abstract level. Semantic meaning refers to symbolic associations, such as the idea that rich people eat caviar. Episodic memories relate to events that are personally relevant to us. As a result, our motivation to retain these memories will be strong. We call especially vivid memories flashbulb memories. For example, where you were when you first heard about the 9/11 attacks could be held in your memory as a flashbulb memory. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Figure 3.5 The Memory Process According to the information-processing perspective, there are three distinct memory systems: sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory. Each plays a role in processing brand-related information, as summarized in the figure. Sensory memory stores the information we receive from our senses. This storage is temporary. If the information is retained for further processing, it passes through an attentional gate and transfers to short-term memory. Short-term memory also stores information for a limited period of time, and it has limited capacity. This system is working memory. It holds information we are currently processing. Our memories can store verbal input acoustically or semantically. We store this information by combining small pieces into larger ones in a process we call chunking. A chunk is a configuration that is familiar and the person can think about it as a unit. Long-term memory is the system that allows us to retain information for a long period of time. A cognitive process we call elaborative rehearsal allows information to move from short-term memory to long-term memory. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Figure 3.6 An Associative Networks for Perfumes Recent research suggests that long-term memory and short-term memory are interdependent systems. Depending on the nature of the processing task, different levels of processing occur that activate some aspects of memory rather than others. These approaches are called activation models of memory. The more effort it takes to process information, the more likely it is that information will transfer into long-term memory. According to these activation models of memory, an incoming piece of information gets stored in an associative network that contains many bits of information. These storage units are knowledge structures – like a complex spider web filled with pieces of data. Incoming information gets put into nodes that connect to one another. Figure 3.6 shows an associative network for perfumes. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Spreading Activation As one node is activated, other nodes associated with it also begin to be triggered Meaning types of associated nodes: Brand-specific Ad-specific Brand identification Product category Evaluative reactions A marketing message may activate our memory of a brand directly or indirectly. If it activates a node, it will also activate other linked nodes much as tapping a spider’s web in one spot sends movement reverberating across the web. The process of spreading activation allows us to shift back and forth among levels of meaning. The way we store a piece of information in memory depends on the type of meaning we initially assign to it. This meaning type then determines how and when something activates the meaning. The meaning types are listed in the slide. Brand-specific meaning refers to memory stored in terms of the claims the brand makes. Ad-specific meaning refers to memories stored in terms of the medium or content of the ad itself. Brand identification is memory stored in terms of the brand name. Product category meaning is memory stored in terms of how the product works or where it should be used. Evaluative reactions is memory stored as positive or negative emotions. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Levels of Knowledge Individual nodes = meaning concepts Two (or more) connected nodes = proposition (complex meaning) Two or more propositions = schema We encode info that is consistent with an existing schema more readily Service scripts Within a knowledge structure, we code elements at different levels of abstraction and complexity. Meaning concepts (like “macho”) get stored as individual nodes. We may combine these concepts into a larger unit we call a proposition or a belief. A proposition links two nodes together to form a more complex meaning. For example, “Axe is cologne for macho men” is a proposition. One type of schema that is especially relevant to consumer behavior is a script. A script is a sequence of events an individual expects to occur. As consumers we learn service scripts that guide our behavior in commercial settings. We expect a certain sequence of events and we may become uncomfortable if the service departs from our script. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Retrieval for Purchase Decisions Retrieving information often requires appropriate factors and cues: Physiological factors Situational factors Consumer attention; pioneering brand; descriptive brand names Viewing environment (continuous activity; commercial order in sequence) Post experience advertising effects Retrieval is the process by which we recover information from long-term memory. There are differences in retrieval ability among people. Individual cognitive or physiological factors account for some of the differences. Others are influenced by situational factors. Even the way the marketer presents the message influences the likelihood that we can recall it later. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

What Makes Us Forget? Appropriate factors/cues for retrieval: State-dependent retrieval/ mood congruence effect Familiarity Salience/von Restorff effect Visual memory versus verbal memory Early memory theorists thought that memories just faded with time. This is known as decay. Forgetting can also occur as a result of interference. Consumers may forget stimulus-response associations if they subsequently learn new responses to the same or similar stimuli. This is called retroactive interference. When prior learning interferes with new learning, this is called proactive interference. It is important for marketers to understand what can help us to remember so that messages can be planned appropriately. The process called state-dependent retrieval means that we are better able to access information if our internal state is the same at the time of recall as when we learned the information. Familiarity and recall states that we are more likely to recall messages about items we are already familiar with. The salience of a brand refers to its prominence or level of activation in memory. Almost any technique that increases the novelty of a stimulus also improves recall. This result is called the von Restorff Effect. This explains why unusual advertising or distinctive packaging tends to facilitate brand recall. The intensity and type of emotions we experience at the time also affect the way we recall the event. We recall mixed emotions differently than unipolar emotions. Unipolar emotions become even more polarized over time. That means that good things seem even better and bad things even worse. The viewing context our ability to remember. That’s why some marketers have begun to utilize hybrid ads. Hybrid ads have some kind of program tie-in to relate the commercial to the program showing. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Measuring Memory for Marketing Stimuli Recognition versus recall Problems with memory measures Response biases Memory lapses Omitting Averaging Telescoping Illusion of truth effect How do we know if our marketing messages are designed to help consumer remember them? We can measure recognition and recall. In a typical recognition test, researchers show ads to subjects one at a time and ask if they have seen them before. In contrast, free recall tests ask consumers to independently think of what they have seen without being prompted first. Of course, measures of memory can be faulty. For one, they may be contaminated by response biases. For example, people tend to give yes responses to questions regardless of the question. People also suffer from memory lapses. Typical problems include omitting facts, averaging (or normalizing the memories by not reporting extreme cases), and telescoping (inaccurate recall of time). The illusion of truth effect may occur as well. This effect refers to the phenomenon of people remembering a claim is true when they have been told the claim is false. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

The Marketing Power of Nostalgia Marketers may resurrect popular characters to evoke fond memories of the past Nostalgia Retro brand Nostalgia describes the emotions where we view the past with longing. We reference the good old days. When marketers play on nostalgia, they want us to attach our fond memories to new products. One way to do this is to introduce retro brands. A retro brand is an updated version of a brand from a prior historical period. The Mini Cooper, PT Cruiser, and Volkswagon’s New Beetle are all retro brands. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Discussion What “retro brands” are targeted to you? Were these brands that were once used by your parents? What newer brands focus on nostalgia, even though they never existed before? 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Chapter Summary Marketers need to know how consumers learn in order to develop effective messages. Conditioning results in learning and learned associations can generalize to other things. Learning can be accomplished through classical and instrumental conditioning and through observing the behavior of others. We use memory systems to store and retrieve information. 4/13/2017 Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall