Describing and exploring the power of relational thinking Max Stephens The University of Melbourne NNFC Auckland Feb. 2007.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Keystone Illustrations Keystone Illustrations next Set 10 © 2007 Herbert I. Gross.
Advertisements

Spring 2012 Student Performance Analysis Algebra I Standards of Learning Presentation may be paused and resumed using the arrow keys or the mouse. 1 Revised.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. 6 Equations and Formulas.
Are You Smart Enough to be a 5 th Grader Teacher?
Algebra Problems… Solutions Algebra Problems… Solutions © 2007 Herbert I. Gross Set 7 part 2 By Herb I. Gross and Richard A. Medeiros next.
Developing a reasoning classroom
Math Department Endeavour Primary School.  Purpose of workshop  Misconceptions and mistakes by topic: ◦ Whole Numbers (P5) ◦ Fractions (P5 and P6) ◦
The Fundamental Property of Rational Expressions
Algebra Problems… Solutions Algebra Problems… Solutions © 2007 Herbert I. Gross Set 4 By Herb I. Gross and Richard A. Medeiros next.
Beyond Right and Wrong Using Open-Ended Questioning in the Mathematics Classroom Jeff Mahood, Colegio Bolívar.
OPAPP “Grazing Area” Task  Class pictures and videos  Student examples of MOODLE assignments, responses, and feedback.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Elaborating responses to fraction assessment tasks reveals students’ algebraic thinking Catherine Pearn Senior Research Fellow
Exam 3 Material Inequalities and Absolute Value
College Algebra Exam 2 Material.
Key Stone Problems… Key Stone Problems… next © 2007 Herbert I. Gross Set 12.
College Algebra Fifth Edition James Stewart Lothar Redlin Saleem Watson.
Multiplying, Dividing, and Simplifying Radicals
The Game of Algebra or The Other Side of Arithmetic The Game of Algebra or The Other Side of Arithmetic © 2007 Herbert I. Gross by Herbert I. Gross & Richard.
Lesson 4: Percentage of Amounts.
Algebra Problems… Solutions
Keystone Problems… Keystone Problems… next Set 19 © 2007 Herbert I. Gross.
Properties refer to rules that indicate a standard procedure or method to be followed. A proof is a demonstration of the truth of a statement in mathematics.
Identity and Equality Properties. Properties refer to rules that indicate a standard procedure or method to be followed. A proof is a demonstration of.
USING NUMBER SENTENCES TO INTRODUCE THE IDEA OF VARIABLE Max Stephens The University of Melbourne NNFC Auckland Feb
Chapter 3: Equations and Inequations This chapter begins on page 126.
1 1.1 © 2012 Pearson Education, Inc. Linear Equations in Linear Algebra SYSTEMS OF LINEAR EQUATIONS.
Critical Thinking Primary Mathematics. Views of Mathematics The way in which we teach mathematics is likely to be influenced by what we think mathematics.
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc
Mathematics Grade 4.
Region 11: Math & Science Teacher Center Solving Equations.
CHAPTER 2 LIMITS AND DERIVATIVES. 2.2 The Limit of a Function LIMITS AND DERIVATIVES In this section, we will learn: About limits in general and about.
Solving Linear Equations = 13 What number would make this equation true? That is, what value of would make the left side equal to the right side?
Multi-digit Numerical Long Division 1 © 2013 Meredith S. Moody.
Have you implemented “Number Talks” in your classroom? What are the pros? What are the cons? Any suggestions?????
Given the marginal cost, find the original cost equation. C ' ( x ) = 9 x 2 – 10 x + 7 ; fixed cost is $ 20. In algebra, we were told that what ever was.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. CHAPTER 5 Extending the Number System.
Variables Tutorial 3c variable A variable is any symbol that can be replaced with a number to solve a math problem. An open sentence has at least one.
Multiplying Whole Numbers © Math As A Second Language All Rights Reserved next #5 Taking the Fear out of Math 9 × 9 81 Single Digit Multiplication.
Chosen Multiplication Methods and the Ability to Learn New Methods Introduction I intended to explore how students thought about the multiplication algorithms.
Algebra Form and Function by McCallum Connally Hughes-Hallett et al. Copyright 2010 by John Wiley & Sons. All rights reserved. 3.1 Solving Equations Section.
1 The role of arithmetic structure in the transition from arithmetic to Algebra Presenters: Wei-Chih Hsu Professor : Ming-Puu Chen Date : 09/02/2008 Warren,
Unaddition (Subtraction)
Identity and Equality Properties. Properties refer to rules that indicate a standard procedure or method to be followed. A proof is a demonstration of.
Dr. John D. Barge, State School Superintendent “Making Education Work for All Georgians” Assessment for Learning Series Module 2: Understanding.
College Algebra Sixth Edition James Stewart Lothar Redlin Saleem Watson.
§ 6.6 Rational Equations. Blitzer, Intermediate Algebra, 5e – Slide #2 Section 6.6 Solving a Rational Equation A rational equation, also called a fractional.
The Commutative Property Using Tiles © Math As A Second Language All Rights Reserved next #4 Taking the Fear out of Math.
Long and Short Term Goals To develop a responsible and positive attitude we chose Respect for Self, Others and Learning for the long term goal. Our students.
WHAT IS BCR and EC R BCR – basic constructed response ECR – extended constructed response.
Section 5.3 The Rational Numbers Math in Our World.
Alternative Algorithms for Addition and Subtraction If we don’t teach them the standard way, how will they learn to compute?
Chapter 2 Real Numbers and algebraic expressions ©2002 by R. Villar All Rights Reserved Re-engineered by Mistah Flynn 2015.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. Fundamental Concepts of Algebra 1.1 Real Numbers.
Adding and Subtracting Decimals © Math As A Second Language All Rights Reserved next #8 Taking the Fear out of Math 8.25 – 3.5.
End of Key Stage 1 Statutory Assessments Teacher Assessment End of KS1 assessment will be based on judgements made by teachers throughout the key.
CHAPTER 3 – UNDERSTANDING WHOLE NUMBER OPERATIONS Reconceptualizing Mathematics Part 1: Reasoning About Numbers and Quantities Judith Sowder, Larry Sowder,
Plenary 1. What’s important about the Math we Teach? A Focus on Big Ideas Marian Small
Identity and Equality Properties. Properties refer to rules that indicate a standard procedure or method to be followed. A proof is a demonstration of.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. 8 9 Correlation and Regression.
Mathematics Section Numbers and Operations Measurement Data Interpretation Algebra Calculators are not allowed on the test!
Next Contents Back. Next Contents Back The Integers are natural numbers including 0 (0, 1, 2, 3,...) and their negatives (0, −1, −2, −3,...). They are.
Precalculus Fifth Edition Mathematics for Calculus James Stewart Lothar Redlin Saleem Watson.
Week 1 Real Numbers and Their Properties (Section 1.6, 1.7, 1.8)
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Identity and Equality Properties
Chapter 7 Objectives Define basic terms in algebra: integer, number statement, expression, and coefficient Learn the relationships between positive and.
Presented by: Angela J. Williams
Equations and Inequalities
Mistakes.
Presentation transcript:

Describing and exploring the power of relational thinking Max Stephens The University of Melbourne NNFC Auckland Feb. 2007

2 Focus for this paper Blanton and Kaput (2001) encourage teachers, especially in the primary school, to grow “algebra eyes and ears” (p. 91). This is not an easy task when teachers’ vision has for so long been restricted to thinking of arithmetic primarily as computation

3 Focus for this paper This paper looks at forms of thinking which do not rely on computation that can be used with addition and subtraction number sentences. It looks at how these forms of thinking are used by students in Years 5 to 7 in two schools, what distinguishes this thinking from computational thinking, and how consistently it is used by students across different problem types.

4 Loretta’s Thinking Loretta has written the following number sentence = She did not have to add up the numbers to know this. Why?

5 Students’ responses One Year 6 student said, “Loretta just knows that they both add up to 63”. A Year 5 student said: “Loretta can do this because she did it in her head”. Neither student could explain why Loretta did not need to add the numbers in order to know that she was correct without using an explanation based on computation.

6 Two other students’ responses to = One Year 5 student drew the following: = Referring to the 29, the student wrote, “It increases by 1 to give 30, so 34 has to decrease by 1 to give 33”. +1 –1

7 Two other students’ responses to = A second Year 5 student inserted an arrow: = No number was attached to the arrow, but the student wrote: “If one unit moves from the 30, the other number becomes 34”

8 Questions used in this study Three groups of missing number sentences: Group A (one page) = 26 +  = 72 +  43 +  =  + 17 =

9 Questions used in this study Three groups of missing number sentences: Group B (one page) 39 – 15 = 41 –  99 –  = 90 – – 45 =  – 46

10 Questions used in this study Three groups of missing number sentences: Group C (spread over two pages) 746 –  =  – 262 = 747

11 Relational thinking Importance of seeing the equal sign as an indication of equivalence or balance Kieran (1981) noted that many children in primary school still think of the equal sign as a direction to find the answer Carpenter and Franke (2001) also argued that understanding equivalence between terms and operations each side of the equal sign enables children to think relationally

12 Computational thinking Successful computational thinkers also rely on a correct understanding of equivalence to solve sentences like =  + 10 They reason that the since two numbers on the left add to 21, the two numbers on the right must also add to 21. Since one number is 10, the missing number has to be 11. This sense of equivalence may be called equivalence of results

13 Relational thinking Relational thinkers use equivalence in another way. They are able to keep the two numbers either side of the equal sign as uncalculated pairs. Collis (1975) refers to this as acceptance of lack of closure They do not rely on equivalence of results, and can work with uncalculated pairs in equivalent expressions

14 Relational thinking It is not correct to say that relational thinkers first abstain from ‘closing’ some operations and then look at the size and direction of the variation to find a missing number Relational thinkers seem to abstain from closing some operations because they see possibilities of variation between the numbers in an equivalence relation where the direction and size of the variation depend on the numbers and operations involved

15 Relational thinking Watson and Mason (2004) point to the importance of enabling students to discern dimensions of variation in which some elements in mathematical sentences change while other elements remain unchanged Discerning possibilities of variation in specified directions seems to be the key to relational thinking

16 Relational thinking Possibilities of variation are more easily discerned when the numbers are close to each other like 99 and 90, or 72 and 73, or 746 and 747. In a number sentence like = 23 +  would you solve it relationally? Of course, it can be solved relationally but would you do it that way or by computation?

17 Relational thinking Equivalence permits but does not define relational thinking. What marks relational thinking is the capacity to use possibilities of variation in number sentences One needs to know the direction in which the missing number will change The directions of variation in = 72 +  are quite different from 90 – 59 = 99 – 

18 Instructions to students 301 students in two schools completed the questions – the questionnaire was given in regular class time. Students were told: “This is not a test. It is a questionnaire prepared by researchers … looking at how students read interpret and understand number sentences. For most of the questions there is more than one way of giving a correct answer. Please write your thinking as clearly as you can. Write your thinking in the space provided after each question and don’t feel that you have to write a lot.”

19 For students who used computational procedures Group A was expected to be easiest, involving an initial addition and then a subtraction Group B was expected to be more difficult, more likely to involve arithmetical errors because students were asked to perform an initial subtraction followed by a second subtraction, and Group C was expected to be the most difficult to solve correctly, involving calculations with three- digit numbers

20 For students who used a relational approach Group A was thought to be easiest Group B was expected to be more difficult, with some students trying to use a relational approach in Group B, but mistaking the direction of variation Group C questions were expected to be less prone to errors, using a relational approach, than approaches based on computation

21 Scoring rubric used a five-point scale 0 – arithmetical thinking evident for all questions; for example, through evidence of progressive calculations and use of algorithms to obtain relevant totals for additions and subtractions, even where these approaches may have resulted in incorrect answers, and no evidence of any relational thinking; also where an answer only has been given without any working shown to indicate what method has been used 1– a clear attempt to use relational thinking in at least one question, but not successfully executed ( e.g. in Group B by giving answers of 13, 15 and 103)

22 Scoring rubric used a five-point scale 2 – relational thinking clearly shown in one question and successfully executed, even if the other problems are solved computationally or using incorrect relational thinking 3 – relational thinking clearly shown in at least two questions and successfully executed, but where the remaining question or questions are not solved relationally or solved using incorrect relational thinking 4 – all questions solved clearly and successfully using relational thinking, computational solutions may also be provided in parallel

23 Group A items

24 Group B items

25 Group C items

26 Instances of clear relational thinking Group A In  + 17 = if I take 2 from 17 and add 2 to 22, it is the same as the number sentence after it. (School A, Year 6) In 43 +  = , 43 to 48 is +5, 81 to 76 is –5. These are equivalent, as you’ve done the same action to both sides. (School A, Year 7)

27 Instances of clear relational thinking Group B 99 –  = 90 – 59 As 99 is 9 more than 90, the missing number must be 9 more than 59. Therefore the answer is 68 (School B, Year 5) 104 – 45 =  – 46 I added 1 to 104 and 45. As long as I add the same number to both, it (104 – 45) will stay equivalent (School B, Year 6)

28 Instances of clear relational thinking Group C  – 262 = is one less than 747, so 262 is one less than the answer. My answer is 263. (School A, Year 5)  – 262 = is I unit less than 747, so if you add 263 you will only need to minus 1 unit less than 263 for the equation to be equal on both sides. (School B, Year 7)

29 Computational responses Group C In Year 5 at School A, among 33 students who tackled Group C using computation, only seven students calculated correct answers to both parts of Group C. Nine students failed to get a correct answer to either part. Seventeen students obtained one correct answer, usually to the first item, 746 –  = 747. The second item  – 262 = 747 proved more difficult for computational thinkers at all Year levels.

30 Stability of thinking How consistent were students in their use of relational or computational thinking across the three Groups of problems? Stable relational – those who used some relational approaches across all three Groups Stable arithmetical – those who used only computational approaches in all three Groups

31 Stability of thinking rubric Not Stable –those whose thinking was not consistent across the three Groups SR: if a student scored ≥ 1 on each of Group A, B, and C SA: if a student scored 0 on each of Group A, B, and C NS: if a student scored ≥ 1 on one or two of Group A, B, or C; and 0 on the other(s).

32 Stability of thinking rubric A criterion of ≥ 1, instead of ≥ 2, across the three groups as evidence of stable relational thinking was justified because a score of 1 on Group B was without exception associated with successful relational thinking (≥ 2) for Group A and/or Group C questions Aside from responses to Group B, a score of 1 was infrequent

33 Stability of students’ responses

34 Results of stability analysis School A(= School 1) The proportion of SR appears to change very little over the three years. However, the biggest difference between Year 5 and Year 6 is the proportion of SA being 15% lower in Year 6, and a corresponding rise in the NS classification. This suggests an emergence of relational thinking between Year 5 and Year 6

35 Results of stability analysis School B(=School 2) The proportion of SR in Year 5 is only slightly higher than that of School A, but in Year 5 there is a higher proportion of students (NS) who show evidence of relational thinking than in School A Comparing Year 6 to Year 5, the proportion of SR is more than double, with a smaller proportion of NS in Year 6 than in Year 5, and only a small drop in the proportion of SA

36 Results of stability analysis School B (contd) The proportion of SA is smaller in Year 7 with a greater proportion classified as SR and NS This may well be explained by explicit teaching of relational approaches in School B by one specialist mathematics teacher who takes classes at all three levels

37 Conclusions Can the use of relational thinking to solve number sentences of the kind shown above can be called algebraic? Pimm (1995) would agree, arguing that “Algebra, right back to its origins, seems to be fundamentally dynamic, operating on transforming form. It is also about equivalence, something is preserved despite apparent change” (p. 88)

38 Conclusions When children show that the numbers in sentences like = can vary without affecting the truth of the expression, they are engaging in generalised numerical thinking which may be called quasi-variable thinking (Fujii & Stephens, 2001) These children may not have learned that is always equivalent to (73 – a) + (49 + a). However, when they create other equivalent arithmetical expressions, they are using some possibilities of variation that can be applied to

39 Conclusions Children who use relational thinking to create equivalent expressions to may not grasp the full range of variation embedded in the formal statement = (73 – a) + (49 + a) where a can represent whole numbers, and rational and negative numbers as well; and where (73 – a) + (49 + a) can also be written as (73 + a) + (49 – a)

40 Conclusions But their successful generation of specific instances using relational thinking can be described as quasi-variable thinking because its range of variation is limited This capacity to generate equivalent numerical expressions makes it different from computation thinking It appears to be a pre-cursor of formal algebraic thinking