Individual Differences in Impulsive-like Behavior & Sensitivity to Money as a Function of Sensation Seeking Status LaBedz, S., Babalonis, S., & Kelly,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Social Learning / Imitation
Advertisements

Predictors of Change in HIV Risk Factors for Adolescents Admitted to Substance Abuse Treatment Passetti, L. L., Garner, B. R., Funk, R., Godley, S. H.,
Effort Discounting of Exam Grades Heidi L. Dempsey, David W. Dempsey, & Arian Ward Jacksonville State University.
Recognition memory amongst individuals varying in the personality dimensions of Reward Seeking and Impulsivity Chase Kluemper 1, Chelsea Black 1, Yang.
Tobacco Use Assessment Life History Questionnaire (LHC): This questionnaire is a retrospective method for collecting data on a wide range of life events.
Results PASAT Mood Manipulation PANAS Outcomes. Results of the ANCOVA with PANAS as the dependent variable revealed a significant main effect for mood.
Ontario`s Mandated High School Community Service Program: Assessing Civic Engagement After Four Years S. D. Brown, S.M. Pancer, P. Padanyi, M. Baetz, J.
Psychological Methods
Method Introduction Discussion Results Discounting of Delayed and Probabilistic Rewards in Gambling and Non-gambling College Students Rochelle R. Smits,
ODAC May 3, Subgroup Analyses in Clinical Trials Stephen L George, PhD Department of Biostatistics and Bioinformatics Duke University Medical Center.
Inhibition as a predictor of performance on an Old/New recognition memory task Chase Kluemper 1, Seth Kiser 1, Yang Jiang 1, Jane E. Joseph 2, & Thomas.
Lectures 15 & 16: Instrumental Conditioning (Schedules of Reinforcement) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
Chapter 6 Variables Used in Experimentation ♣ ♣ Types of Variables   The Independent Variable   The Dependent Variable   Demo: Identifying IVs and.
263 US residents completed the study over the internet, making hypothetical choices between immediate and future monetary and environmental gains (within-subjects.
Methylphenidate Self-Administration in High- and Low-Impulsive Sensation Seekers Using a Progressive-Ratio Procedure Lee, D.C., Robbins, G., Martin, C.A.,
Quantitative Research
Table 1 Summarizes demographic and drug-use variables among light and heavy marijuana users. Significant group differences were observed on marijuana use.
Negative Urgency, Distress Tolerance and Problematic Alcohol Use Abstract Purpose: This study aimed to explore the relations among Negative Urgency, Distress.
Jared A. Rowland, M.S., Michael M. Knepp, M.S., Sheri L. Towe, M.S., Chris S. Immel, M.S., Ryoichi J.P. Noguchi, M.S., Chad L. Stephens, M.S. & David W.
Collecting Quantitative Data
Chapter 9 Adjusting to Schedules of Partial Reinforcement.
APA Method Section.  Things to keep in mind...  Purpose: Replication Assess reliability & validity Make design clear Keep it precise, concise, and clear.
Chapter 13: Schedules of Reinforcement
Introduction ► College-student drinking remains a significant problem on campuses across the nation. ► It is estimated that 38-44% of college students.
METHODS Sample n=245 Women, 24% White, 72% Average age, 36.5 Never married, 51% Referral Sources (%) 12-Month DSM-IV Substance Dependence Prior to Entering.
Participants: Participants consisted of 26 (n = 26), healthy, college participants (5 males and 21 females) aged years. See Table 1. Protocol:
Decision Making in Students Differing in Binge Drinking Patterns Anna E. Goudriaan, Emily R. Grekin, and Kenneth J. Sher University of Missouri-Columbia.
Temporal Discounting of Various Items to Examine Characteristics that Affect Rate of Discounting Kathryn R. Haugle, Rochelle R. Smits, & Daniel D. Holt.
Reinforcers and Punishers versus Incentives Reinforcers and punishers refer to good and bad behavior consequences.
Can Money Buy Happiness? Evidence from the Discounting of Uncertain Happiness Tracy A. Tufenk & Daniel D. Holt Psychology Department, University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Quality Of Life, Health And Well Being Of Highly Active Individuals Louisa Raisbeck, Jeanne Johnston, Joel Stager, Francoise Benay Human Performance Laboratory,
LINKING PSYCHOMETRIC RISK TOLERANCE WITH CHOICE BEHAVIOUR FUR Conference – July 2008 Peter Brooks, Greg B. Davies and Daniel P. Egan.
Introduction Results A New Method for Quantifying Outcomes in Discounting Rochelle R. Smits, Matthew H. Newquist & Daniel D. Holt University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Chapter 1 Introduction to Statistics. Statistical Methods Were developed to serve a purpose Were developed to serve a purpose The purpose for each statistical.
Introduction Impulsivity has been defined in various ways, and broadly “refers to factors that regulate the performance of inappropriate or maladaptive.
Investigating the Step Size in a Progressive-Ratio Schedule of Reinforcement for Young Children Diagnosed with Autism Kathryn R. Glodowski, Chelsea B.
Temperament and Character Adaptations to Addictions Treatment Daniel Angres, M.D.¹ &Stephanie Bologeorges, B.A.¹ ¹ Resurrection Behavioral Health-Addiction.
Introduction The authors of this research would like to thank the University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire’s Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for financial.
Temporal Discounting of Hypothetical and Real Extra Credit Points Makenzie D. Williams, Heidi L. Dempsey, & David W. Dempsey Jacksonville State University.
Deficient feedback processing during risky decision-making in adolescents with a parental history of Substance Use Disorders Anja Euser Erasmus University.
A Comparison of Lifestyle Behaviors Between Student-Athletes and Non-Athletes Alyssa Stefanadis & Eddie T. C. Lam Department of Health & Human Performance,
Introduction Relationship between Extraversion and Delay Discounting of Social Interactions Sara L. Daugherty and Daniel D. Holt University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Introduction Studies are important for gathering information. In this lesson, you will learn how to effectively design a study so that it yields reliable.
Negative Psychosocial Outcomes of Engaging in Sexual Intercourse Before the Age of 16 Introduction Mary K. Higgins Mary K. Higgins,
Understanding Quantitative Research Design
Heidi L. Dempsey, David W. Dempsey, Tomesha Manora, Amanda Webster, Jody Thompson, Aaron Garrett, Iyanna Cammack, Yawa Dossou, Angel Johnston, & Michael.
Temporal Discounting of Various Gift Cards Kathryn R. Glodowski, Rochelle R. Smits, & Daniel D. Holt Psychology Department, University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Individual Differences in Alcohol Effects Among High- and Low- Impulsive Sensation Seekers Kelly, T.H., Robbins, G., Martin, C.A., Marczinski, C.A. and.
Foundations of Research Survey Research This is a PowerPoint Show Open it as a show by going to “slide show”. Click through it by pressing any key.
Introduction ► Despite efforts to reduce heavy drinking among college students, college-student alcohol use and its negative consequences remains a concern.
Drug abuse liability is associated with higher reward-sensitivity: An fMRI study using the Monetary Incentive Delay task C. Corbly, T. Kelly, Y. Jiang,
Method Introduction Results Discussion Mean Negative Cigarette Systoli Previous research has reported that across the nation 29% of college students engage.
Neural Activation for Emotional Induction: Differential Responses as a Function of Impulsivity and Reward-Sensitivity C.E.Benca 1, H.R. Collins 1, C.R.
Parallel Temporal & Probabilistic Discounting of Costs Stephen Jones & Mike Oaksford July 2009.
Within behavioral and economic fields, there are several distinct methods for determining the value of a commodity or reinforcer. Two of these methods.
Research on the relationship between childhood sleep problems and substance use in adolescents and young adults is limited. This knowledge gap has been.
A Psychophysical Approach to Discounting: Sex and Money
Sharon Sznitman & Batya Engel-Yeger
Justin Yates & Michael Bardo
Choice Behavior Two.
My, But We are Impressive
Matthew H. Newquist and Daniel D. Holt
Facets of Impulsivity as Unique Predictors of Substance Use and Abuse
Ontario`s Mandated High School Community Service Program: Assessing Civic Engagement After Four Years S. D. Brown, S.M. Pancer, P. Padanyi, M. Baetz, J.
Volume 15, Issue 7, Pages (April 2005)
Dopamine Reward Prediction Error Responses Reflect Marginal Utility
An Introduction to Correlational Research
Neural Activation for Emotional Induction: Differential Responses as a Function of Impulsivity and Reward-Sensitivity  C.E.Benca1, H.R. Collins1, C.R.
Wallis, JD Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute UC, Berkeley
TEST FOR RANDOMNESS: THE RUNS TEST
Presentation transcript:

Individual Differences in Impulsive-like Behavior & Sensitivity to Money as a Function of Sensation Seeking Status LaBedz, S., Babalonis, S., & Kelly, T.H. University of Kentucky Abstract Previous research indicates that high sensation seekers are at increased vulnerability to drug abuse relative to low sensation seekers. This enhanced risk has been characterized by earlier initiation and greater frequency of drug use among high sensation-seeking adolescents, and increased sensitivity to the reinforcing and other behavioral effects of drugs in laboratory studies, such that high sensation seekers exhibit higher break-points on progressive ratio schedules maintained by drug delivery. The present study examined sensitivity to a generalized reinforcer (i.e., money) and impulsive-like behavior as a function of sensation-seeking status among healthy young adults. Twenty participants scoring in the top and bottom quartiles of gender-adjusted population norms on the impulsive-sensation seeking scale of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (10 high- and 10 low-impulsive sensation seekers) completed one session in which performance on several behavioral tasks was assessed. Participants completed a progressive ratio task in which they could earn up to $4.00 (in.50 increments) by completing progressively increasing response requirements. Other measures included performance on the Balloon Analog Risk Task, a hypothetical delay-discounting task, and a delay-discounting task with a lottery outcome. Breakpoints on the progressive ratio task did not vary as a function of sensation-seeking status. Likewise, performance on behavioral measures of impulsivity did not vary between high and low sensation seekers. These data suggest that group differences in drug-maintained behavior between low and high sensation seekers are not observed when behavior is maintained by money. Moreover, sensation-seeking status was not associated with performance on any laboratory measure of impulsivity (delay discounting, BART). Supported by DA-05312, DA , University of Kentucky Department of Behavioural Science. Results Background 1.The sensation seeking personality trait is described as an inclination towards intense emotional experiences and/or situations and the pursuit of risky or impulsive behavior in order to achieve the sensation (Zuckerman, 1994). 2.Previous studies have shown that a high sensation seeking status may increase an individual’s susceptibility to drug abuse (Wills et al., 1994). 3.Previous laboratory research has shown that the reinforcing and behavioral effects of drugs (d-amphetamine, alcohol, nicotine) are enhanced in high sensation seekers, relative to low sensation seekers (Stoops et al., 2007; unpublished data), indicating that high sensation-seekers may be more vulnerable to the reinforcing effects of drugs, and thereby might be at greater risk for developing repeated patterns of drug-seeking behavior. 4.The purpose of the present study is to determine if the reinforcing effects of a generalized reinforcer (i.e. money) and performance on laboratory measures of impulsive-like behavior will differ between high and low sensation seekers. Methods Participants: Twenty, non-smoking healthy adults, ages 19 to 32, gave written consent prior to participating in a single session lasting approximately 2.5 hours. Ten participants were classified as High Sensation Seekers, and ten were classified as Low Sensation Seekers, with each group having equal numbers of male and female participants. All participants provided drug-free urine and alcohol-free breath samples prior to participation. All subjects received task training and practice prior to the experimental session and were paid for their participation upon completion of the session. Sensation-Seeking Status: Volunteers completed items from the impulsive sensation-seeking scale of the Zuckerman- Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire. Those who scored in the upper and lower 25% of the population, based on established norms, were classified as High and Low Sensation Seekers, respectively, and invited to participate. Assessment Tasks Progressive Ratio Task: This task consisted of eight consecutive opportunities to earn money in increments of $0.50 by responding on a computer mouse. Participants could earn none, some, or all of the available money ($4.00). To earn the first $0.50, participants were required to click the mouse 50 times. The requirement then doubled for each additional $0.50 (e.g., 100, 200, 400, 800, 1600, 3200, and 6400 clicks), such that 12,750 were required to earn all of the available money ($4.00). However, the participant could choose to stop responding at any time, either by selecting “no” when asked if they wished to continue after earning an increment of money or by stopping clicking at any time (e.g., an IRT ≥ 2 minutes ended the task). The dependent measures of interest during this task ware break-point (the last ratio completed) and concomitantly, the amount of money earned. Hypothetical Delay Discounting Task: A series of hypothetical choices were presented, positing a choice between two options: an immediate, smaller amount of money and a larger, delayed amount of money. The immediate amount was increased until preference between the two options reached indifference. The hypothetical delayed money option was fixed ($1000) and was presented at 7 delay values (1 week, 2 weeks, 1 mo, 6 mo, 1 yr, 5 yr and 25 yr). The dependent measure was indifference point for each delay value (I.e., the immediate amount of money that shifts preference from the larger, delayed option). Participants were instructed that each of their choices were hypothetical and they would not be paid for any of their selections. Real Delay Discounting Task: A series of choices were presented, positing a choice between two options: an immediate, smaller amount of money and a larger, delayed amount of money. The delayed money option was fixed ($20) and was presented at 10 delay values (1 day, 3 days, 5 days, 1 week, 10 days, 2 weeks, 3 weeks, 25 days, 1 month, and 2 months). The immediate amount ($20, $18, $15, $12, $10, $8, $6, $4, $2, and $1), was increased until preference between the immediate amount and the delayed amount ($20) reached indifference. This indifference point served as the dependent measure. Of the 200 choices the participants made, they were paid for one randomly selected choice (ex.: $15 in 2 weeks). Balloon Analog Task: This task simulated a balloon being inflated in small increments controlled by clicking on a computer mouse (e.g., Lejuez et al., 2003). On each trial a participant decided to inflate the balloon or move to another balloon. A successful inflation resulted in a monetary increment to a temporary bank and an increase in the probability of the balloon popping on the next inflation. If a participant choose to move to another balloon, the amount in the temporary bank was placed in a permanent bank; if a participant choose to inflate the balloon and it popped, money in the temporary bank was lost. Progressive Ratio Delay Discounting Tasks Balloon Analog Risk Task Figure 1. The amount of money earned (left panel) and total number of clicks emitted (right panel) on the Progressive Ratio Task as a function of sensation-seeking status. No significant differences between groups were detected in either measure. Earnings ($) Number of Pops Responses per Balloon Number of Clicks Sensation Seeking Status Time (days) Subjective value ($20) Subjective Value ($1000) Figure 2. Mean discounting curves generated under real (left panel) and hypothetical (right panel) conditions when indifference points are fit to the hyperbolic discounting function [A = V/(1+kD)]. Both graphs display prototypical discounting functions, with the subjective value of money decreasing as a function of delay to its delivery. No differences in discounting functions between groups were detected in either task. 1.Progressive Ratio Task- This task has been used in a variety of experimental settings to examine behavioral sensitivity to the reinforcing effects of a stimulus. The breakpoint, i.e. the last ratio completed, generally serves as the dependent measure of reinforcing efficacy. The selected ratio value was based on previous research (Stoops et al., 2007). These contingencies engendered variability in responding; however, breakpoint did not vary as a function of sensation seeking status. Previous studies have shown that performance on this task maintained by d-amphetamine administration differs as a function on sensation seeking status. Money served as a reinforcer in both high and low sensation seekers; however, there were no differences in breakpoints as a function of sensation seeking status. 2.Hypothetical Delay Discounting Task- This task has been used to characterize behavioral sensitivity to reinforcer delay, such that a value of a reinforcer decreases as the delay to its delivery increases. Performance on this task differs as a function of current drug or alcohol use, gambling habits, smoking status, and age of first use of alcohol or illicit substance (see Bickel & Marsch, 2001; Reynolds, 2006). Prototypical discounting functions were generated in this task; however, there were no significant differences in indifference points as a function of sensation seeking status. 3.Real Delay Discounting Task- Similar to the hypothetical task, this task generates behavior that differs as a function of particular behavioral histories (i.e., drug use, gambling, etc.) This task differs from the hypothetical task by introducing contingencies, such that one choice is randomly selected and rewarded (e.g., $15 delivered in 10 days). Prototypical discounting functions were generated in this task; however, there were no significant differences in indifference points as a function of sensation seeking status. 4.Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART)- This task has been used to examine sensitivity to reward and inhibitory control. Previous studies have shown that performance on this task differs as a function of drug abuse history, smoking status, and presence of behavioral problems (Lejuez et al., 2002, 2003, 2007). Prototypical behavior was emitted on the BART; however behavior did not vary as a function of sensation-seeking status. Conclusions Sensation-Seeking Status 1.There were no differences detected in behavioral sensitivity to a generalized reinforcer or delay to a reinforcer as a function of sensation seeking status. 2.Behavioral effects were not significantly different with respect to other factors such as gender, age, and education. 3.The lack of significant difference on performance task measures between the personality groups may be attributed to the heterogeneous nature of impulsivity and the reliability of quantitative measures of personality as a predictor of behavior. 4.Previous studies have shown that the reinforcing effects of drugs differ between high and low sensation seekers. However, no differences were observed when a generalized reinforcer was available. These results suggest that differential sensitivity to reinforcers as a function of sensation seeking status may be specific to certain commodities (i.e., drugs). Figure 3. Mean number of responses per balloon on balloons that did not pop (left panel); mean number of popped balloons (middle panel) and mean earnings as a function of sensation-seeking status. Each bar represents the mean of three task presentations with error bars representing +- one standard error of the mean. No practice effects were detected across the three task presentations for either group and no significant differences between groups were detected on any measure.