Chance REORIENTATION BY SLOPE CUES IN HUMANS Introduction Daniele Nardi, Amanda Y. Funk, Nora S. Newcombe & Thomas F. Shipley Temple University Animals.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Drawing In One-Point Perspective
Advertisements

ME 101 ENGINEERING GRAPHICS Isometric Drawing
ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTION
Using.  Purpose ▪ What question were the authors trying to answer? ▪ What was the primary relationship being studied?  Methods ▪ Subtitles: Participants;
Comparison of Spatial and Temporal Discrimination Performance across Various Difficulty Levels J.E. THROPP, J.L. SZALMA, & P.A. HANCOCK Department of Psychology.
Descriptive Geometry:
1 NOTATIONS FOLLOWING NOTATIONS SHOULD BE FOLLOWED WHILE NAMEING DIFFERENT VIEWS IN ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTIONS. IT’S FRONT VIEW a’ a’ b’ OBJECT POINT A LINE.
The Effects of Vicarious Learning with Experienced Rats Jessie Young and Lyndsey Wells Randolph College Lynchburg, VA Introduction Vicarious learning.
Advanced Visualization Techniques
Navigating with fingers and feet: Comparative analysis of human and rat movement kinematics during non-visual spatial tasks J.R. Raines, N.D. McNeal, J.L.
Product Perception By Michelle Olguin & Karen Dambroski Undergraduate Students Fort Lewis College Durango, Colorado.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS REFERENCES -Are view-boundaries special – eliciting extrapolation? There are other boundaries within a view that have similar characteristics,
Chapter 7 Correlational Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian
ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTION
Pilot: Customizing a Commercially Available Digital Game to Assess Cognitive Function William C. M. Grenhart, John F. Sprufera, Jason C. Allaire, & Anne.
Individual Differences in Spatial Perception and Cognition: Mary Hegarty University of California, Santa Barbara.
ISOMETRICS Isometric means “equal in measure” and refers to the fact that the three receding axes are tilted at 30°. Isometric drawings are constructed.
EMSE 3123 Math and Science in Education
 Pearce et al. (2006, Experiment 1) found potentiation of geometry learning by a feature in a rectangular enclosure, and overshadowing of geometry by.
Implicit Relational Learning in a Multiple-Object Tracking Task: Do People Really Track the Objects? Tiffany Williams and Olga Lazareva (Department of.
Early Arbitrary Object Memory May Set the Stage for Episodic Memory in Toddlers Frances Balcomb, Nora S. Newcombe, Katrina Ferrara, Jule Grant, Sarah M.
ENTC 1110 OBLIQUE PICTORIALS.
Gender Schema Influence Analogical Problem Solving and Memory in Preschool-Aged Children. Gender Schema Influence Analogical Problem Solving and Memory.
Spatial Learning and Memory in the Sand Maze: Does WIN-2 Produce State Dependent Retention in Rats? Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard Randolph-Macon.
Understanding Visualization through Spatial Ability Differences Maria C. Velez, Deborah Silver and Marilyn Tremaine Rutgers University 2005.
Suppression Task. We used a task modeled on Gernsbacher et. al. (1991) Lewis and colleagues 3,4 measured the monocular visual field extent of 3-, 4-, and.
Reicher (1969): Word Superiority Effect Dr. Timothy Bender Psychology Department Missouri State University Springfield, MO
The Relationship Between Geometric Shape and Slope for the Representation of a Goal Location in Pigeons (Columba livia) Daniele Nardi Bowling Green State.
Goal and Motivation To study our (in)ability to detect inconsistencies in the illumination of objects in images Invited Talk! – Hany Farid: Photo Forensincs:
F.V. and S.V.of an object are given. Draw it’s isometric view.
I can be You: Questioning the use of Keystroke Dynamics as Biometrics —Paper by Tey Chee Meng, Payas Gupta, Debin Gao Presented by: Kai Li Department of.
Children Getting Lost: Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans.
ACT Science ACT Test Prep Goals – 1. Become familiar with many of the concepts that are tested on the official test 2. Be able to target the item-types.
1 1 Spatialized Haptic Rendering: Providing Impact Position Information in 6DOF Haptic Simulations Using Vibrations 9/12/2008 Jean Sreng, Anatole Lécuyer,
REFERENCES Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Troetschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit.
I can be You: Questioning the use of Keystroke Dynamics as Biometrics Tey Chee Meng, Payas Gupta, Debin Gao Ke Chen.
PUTTING TO A BIGGER HOLE: GOLF PERFORMANCE RELATES TO PERCEIVED SIZE Erika Larose CLPS 1500 Jessica Witt, 2008.
In both active and passive groups, the correlation between spatial ability and performance was attenuated, relative to previous studies In contrast to.
Disrupting face biases in visual attention Anna S. Law, Liverpool John Moores University Stephen R. H. Langton, University of Stirling Introduction Method.
Effect of laterality-specific training on visual learning Jenna Kelly & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
How we actively interpret our environment..  Perception: The process in which we understand sensory information.  Illusions are powerful examples of.
It is believed that when inter-trial time is not controlled, the CI effect will occur (i.e., random practice will outperform blocked practice in retention),
Earth's magnetic field Measuring the Earth's magnetic field at a given location.
Kimron Shapiro & Frances Garrad-Cole The University of Wales, Bangor
Colour Discrimination Task
Mental Rotation of Naturalistic Human Faces
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
Visual Memory is Superior to Auditory Memory
Table 1. Mean Response Times for Local and Global
Which of these is “a boy”?
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
Are Men Better Visualizers?
Alternate Views Introduction to Engineering Design
Intuitive Perspective
Day 1 – Definition of angles
KS4 Mathematics Linear Graphs.
VISUAL DEPENDENCE IN POSTURAL CONTROL AND SPATIAL ORIENTATION Massimo Cenciarini1, Patrick J. Loughlin1,2, Mark S. Redfern1,3, Patrick J. Sparto1,3 Depts.
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e6 (April 2018)
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTION
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTION
Stephanie Lidd and Jamie Tauber
A8 Linear and real-life graphs
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
Geometry Point Line Line segment Ray Plane Parallel lines
Presentation transcript:

Chance REORIENTATION BY SLOPE CUES IN HUMANS Introduction Daniele Nardi, Amanda Y. Funk, Nora S. Newcombe & Thomas F. Shipley Temple University Animals are remarkably skilled in their capacity to use a range of environmental features to orient in space once they have lost track of where they are, a process called reorientation. Among the types of cues that animals can use, one that has received little attention is the directional information extractable from a surface extending in the vertical dimension, such as a geographical slant or slope (Restat, Steck, Mochnatzki, & Mallot, 2004; Miniaci, Scotto, & Bures, 1999; Proffitt, Bhalla, Gossweiler & Midgett, 1995). Slope is a perceptually salient cue because it provides potentially redundant, multimodal sensory activations, such as visual, proprioceptive, kinesthetic and vestibular stimuli. However, studies have shown that the conscious awareness of slope seems to be highly variable, depending, for example, on physiological and psychosocial resources (Proffitt, Bhalla, Gossweiler & Midgett, 1995; Schnall, Harber, Stefanucci & Proffitt, 2008). The present research aimed to study, for the first time in a real-world environment, the extent to which humans can use a geographical slant for successful reorientation and goal location. Method Apparatus The experimental enclosure consisted of a square, wooden platform with a PVC pipe frame on top (overall size of the enclosure: base, 244 x 244 cm; height, 206 cm; see Fig.1-3). Curtains were placed around and on top of the PVC frame, such that the experimental environment was completely enclosed. The enclosure could be placed horizontally on the floor, or could be tilted to a 5° inclination. Illumination was provided by four 25-W lamps on the platform, one in each corner. Four red bowls, used as potential hiding places for the target, were located in the corners. A swivel chair was placed in the center of the enclosure (even when the enclosure was slanted, the chair’s axis of rotation was always perpendicular to the earth). Training 20 males and 20 females took part to the experiment. The enclosure was slanted with a 5° inclination. The participant saw the experimenter hide the target (a $1 bill) under one of the four bowls. Then the participant was blindfolded and spun on the swivel chair for 1 minute. During this time, the experimenter moved around the curtains so that subjects could not use the wrinkles to determine the goal location. After being disoriented, participants had to tell where the target was hidden. PVC pipe frame Wooden platform ON A HORIZONTAL SURFACEWITH A 5° SLOPE The enclosure was completely symmetrical and featureless, and no external cue could be seen or heard (participants wore earphones). Therefore, the only way to encode and locate the goal was using the slope gradient, i.e., encoding the goal with respect to the vertical and orthogonal (left-right) axes of the slope (e.g., the target is in the upper-left bowl; see Fig. 2). Training was composed of 4 trials, with the target always in the same place. Subjects were given feedback and shown the target at the end of the trial. On each trial subjects started facing a different side of the enclosure. UPHILL DOWNHILL Participant Bowl Testing Subjects left the enclosure and came back two times for the tests (one trial each, with no feedback). Tests were presented in counterbalanced order. Flat test. For one trial the enclosure was placed horizontally on the floor → No polarizing cues were available that would enable to distinguish a specific corner → Expected performance is at chance (25%). Slope test. For one trial the enclosure was tilted again at a 5° inclination → Participants could now locate the target. Results Fig. 4 Mean correct choices (± SD) during the 4 training trials. Both genders performed above chance, males: t(19) = 14.33, p <.0001; females: t(19) = 2.41, p <.05. However, males performed substantially and significantly better than females, t(38) = 4.43, p < This effect size is large, d = 1.4, with means differing for more than 1 SD. Conclusions Both males and females are able to use the slope of the floor to reorient and locate a hidden goal. However, there is a large gender difference in performance because females are less likely to spontaneously use slope cues than males. Males also outperform females in the water-level task, and a similar result has been shown in other visual perception tasks that require engaging the horizontal-vertical reference frame ( Linn & Peterson, 1985 ). However, the present finding is important because it shows a male superiority in the use of a multimodal cue in which kinesthetic and vestibular information – as opposed to visual information – play a crucial role. Future research will have to investigate the factors underlying the gender difference in slope use, and whether this female deficit might be eliminated by drawing more attention to the fact that the floor is tilted. This research was supported in part by National Science Foundation funding of the Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center (SBE ). For further information please contact More information on this and related projects can be found at Fig. 1 Fig. 2Fig. 3 Acute Obtuse Chance Fig. 5 Number of correct choices during the tests. As expected, when tested on a flat surface (i.e., without any cue), participants were disoriented and could not locate the correct corner more than by chance. However, when tested on the slope, both males and females performed significantly above chance (binomial test, males: p <.0001; females: p <.05). These results rule out the possibility that participants were using cues other than the slope to solve the task. Only when the enclosure was slanted could subjects reorient and successfully locate the target. Females Males Fig. 6 Participant self-report strategy use. More males than females reported using slope to locate the target, Fisher’s exact test, p <.05. This suggests that males outperform females because they are more likely to use slope to solve the task. At the end of the experiment subjects completed the water-level task (Piaget & Inhelder, 1956) and reported what information they used to locate the target: -Slope (using the slope gradient) -Other (using path integration or any feature cues, e.g., wrinkles in the curtains) -View-based (using the different angle of incidence between the uphill and downhill curtains with the platform). Male scores in the water-level task were significantly higher than female’s, t(38) = 2.13, p <.05. There was a significant correlation between water-level and slope task performance, r(38) =.36, p <.05. However, when controlling for gender, the correlation was not significant, r(37) =.22, p =.18. This suggests that water-level and slope task performance correlate because males are better than females in both tasks.