VS131 Visual Neuroscience Extra-Striate Cortex Hey stupid: remember to distribute the handouts this time!

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Perception Chapter 4 Visual Process beyond the Retina
Advertisements

Chapter 4: The Visual Cortex and Beyond
The Primary Visual Cortex
Sensory systems in the brain The visual system. Organization of sensory systems PS 103 Peripheral sensory receptors [ Spinal cord ] Sensory thalamus Primary.
Higher Visual Areas Anatomy of higher visual areas
Attention and neglect.
Midterm 1 Oct. 21 in class. Read this article by Wednesday next week!
Central Visual Processes. Anthony J Greene2 Central Visual Pathways I.Primary Visual Cortex Receptive Field Columns Hypercolumns II.Spatial Frequency.
Perception Chapter 9: Event Perception Event Perception: an event is defined as a change in both time and space. Thus far we have discussed how our visual.
EXAM 2 !!! Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday of NEXT WEEK.
Visual Attention Attention is the ability to select objects of interest from the surrounding environment A reliable measure of attention is eye movement.
Chapter 4: Cortical Organization
Human (ERP and imaging) and monkey (cell recording) data together 1. Modality specific extrastriate cortex is modulated by attention (V4, IT, MT). 2. V1.
Read Land’s article about color vision for Tuesday next week.
Extra Credit for Participating in Experiments Go to to sign upwww.tatalab.ca We are recruiting for a variety of experiments including basic.
Anatomy/Physiology of Binocular Vision Goals –Follow the M and P pathway out of primary visual cortex –Answer where binocularly and disparity driven cells.
1.Exams due 9am 16 th. (grades due 10am 19 th ) 2.Describe the organization of visual signals in extra-striate visual cortex and the specialization of.
Binocular Disparity points (C) nearer than fixation (P) have crossed disparity points (F) farther than fixation have uncrossed disparity.
Higher Processing of Visual Information: Lecture III
Visual Neuron Responses This conceptualization of the visual system was “static” - it did not take into account the possibility that visual cells might.
The Apparatus. Seeing in Stereo It’s very hard to read words if there are multiple images on your retina.
Visual Pathways W. W. Norton Primary cortex maintains distinct pathways – functional segregation M and P pathways synapse in different layers Ascending.
Searching for the NCC We can measure all sorts of neural correlates of these processes…so we can see the neural correlates of consciousness right? So what’s.
Read Lamme (2000) TINS article for Wednesday. Visual Pathways V1 is, of course, not the only visual area (it turns out it’s not even always “primary”)
Fundraiser Concert for Haiti U of L Students and Faculty Performing –9pm Blues Sen-sa-shun –10pm Shawna Romolliwa –11pm Dave Renter At The Slice, Friday.
Blue= rods Green = Cones Pathways from the Retina In the brain, retinal ganglion axons travel to… –the hypothalamus: control bodily rhythms.
1 Biological Neural Networks Example: The Visual System.
Read Pinker article for Thurs.. Seeing in Stereo.
How does the visual system represent visual information? How does the visual system represent features of scenes? Vision is analytical - the system breaks.
Today: Finish Gregory Discussion, finish stereograms Tuesday: Start colour vision Thursday: Finish colour vision - Read LAND for Thursday.
Visual Cognition I basic processes. What is perception good for? We often receive incomplete information through our senses. Information can be highly.
Color vision Different cone photo- receptors have opsin molecules which are differentially sensitive to certain wavelengths of light – these are the physical.
Binocular Disparity points nearer than horopter have crossed disparity
Stereoscopic Depth Disparity between the two retinal images indicates an objects distance from the plane of fixation.
PY202 Overview. Meta issue How do we internalise the world to enable recognition judgements to be made, visual thinking, and actions to be executed.
Chapter 10 The Central Visual System. Introduction Neurons in the visual system –Neural processing results in perception Parallel pathway serving conscious.
Higher Processing of Visual Information: Lecture II
Infinity of Interpretations There are an infinite number of interpretations of the 2D pattern of light on the retina.
Laurent Itti: CS599 – Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision 2 1 Computational Architectures in.
Copyright © 2007 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3e Chapter 10: The Central Visual System.
Careers for Psychology and Neuroscience Majors Oct. 19th5-7pm in SU 300 Ballroom B.
Vision. Light is electromagnetic energy. One nm = one billionth of a meter The Visible Spectrum.
Higher Visual Processing
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 3 Neurons Central Visual Pathways See Reading Assignment on "Assignments page"
THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EYE TO CORTEX Outline 1. The Eyes a. Structure b. Accommodation c. Binocular Disparity 2. The Retina a. Structure b. Completion c. Cone.
The Visual Cortex: Anatomy
Slide 1 Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3rd Ed, Bear, Connors, and Paradiso Copyright © 2007 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Bear: Neuroscience: Exploring.
1 Perception, Illusion and VR HNRS 299, Spring 2008 Lecture 8 Seeing Depth.
Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion
Figure 2.7. Number of neural impulses in selected single cells of the monkey brain when shown differing pictures. These neurons fire the most when a face.
Chapter 3: Neural Processing and Perception. Neural Processing and Perception Neural processing is the interaction of signals in many neurons.
Autostereograms Convergence is on a point at the same distance as the images Boxes and faces are on the horopter How many boxes would you see? boxes and.
Higher Visual Areas 1.Anatomy of higher visual areas 2.Two processing pathways - “ Where ” pathway for motion and depth - “ What ” pathway for form and.
Perception and VR MONT 104S, Fall 2008 Lecture 8 Seeing Depth
Week 4 Motion, Depth, Form: Cormack Wolfe Ch 6, 8 Kandell Ch 27, 28 Advanced readings: Werner and Chalupa Chs 49, 54, 57.
1 Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Chapter 19 Visual Network Moran Furman.
Outline Of Today’s Discussion 1.Some Disparities are Not Retinal: Pulfrich Effect 2.Random-Dot Stereograms 3.Binocular Rivalry 4.Motion Parallax.
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 16 Stereopsis.
Figure 12.1 Central projections of retinal ganglion cells
Binocular Disparity points nearer than horopter have crossed disparity points farther than horopter have uncrossed disparity.
Chapter 4: Cortical Organization
A cerebral hemisphere is defined as one of the two regions of the brain that are delineated by the body's median plane.
CHAPTER 10 Vision and visual perception Form Vision.
Chapter 2 Cognitive Neuroscience. Some Questions to Consider What is cognitive neuroscience, and why is it necessary? How is information transmitted from.
1 Perception and VR MONT 104S, Spring 2008 Lecture 3 Central Visual Pathways.
Vision III: Cortical mechanisms of vision
Brodmann’s Areas. Brodmann’s Areas The Primary Visual Cortex Hubel and Weisel discovered simple, complex and hypercomplex cells in the striate.
Optic Nerve Projections
The Visual System: Higher Cortical Mechanisms
Neural Mechanisms of Visual Motion Perception in Primates
Presentation transcript:

VS131 Visual Neuroscience Extra-Striate Cortex Hey stupid: remember to distribute the handouts this time!

Grossly simplified block-diagram of the cortical visual system

Another grossly simplified block- diagram of the cortical visual system

Even more grossly simplified block- diagram of the cortical visual system! You will never see the cortical visual system drawn the same way twice. You have been warned!

Nota Bene: Almost all of our current knowledge of extrastriate visual cortex comes from work in the rhesus monkey. All current textbooks refer to monkey data. Recently, work using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), which unlike other research techniques can be applied in both humans and primates, has started to tie the monkey data to the human data. It appears that, indeed, much of the basic organization of the cortical visual system in monkeys is the same as that of humans. Work is ongoing and as usual there will doubtless be differences between the species, especially in ‘higher’ (farther away from V1) visual cortical areas. Often you may read of the “human homolog of V4” instead of just V4.

Rhesus monkey cortical visual areas unfolded

Human visual cortical areas have about the same layout as the rhesus monkey, but are only about 20% of cortical surface

A neurologists’ view of cerebral cortex (does not cover specific areas): Occipital: VISION Parietal: VISION + integrating sensory information different parts of the body and motor control + stuff Temporal: VISION + hearing + speech/language + memory + stuff Frontal: Motor cortex, “executive function” (planning), + stuff + vision esp. VOLUNTARY SACCADIC EYE MOVEMENTS.

Very crudely: DORSAL PATHWAY: “WHERE” stream of visual processing: magno. VENTRAL PATHWAY: “WHAT” stream of visual processing: parvo.

V2 -> Closely associated with V1 -> BOTH magno and parvo sections -> Neurons in V2 have, at first glance, roughly similar properties to those of neurons in V1. Small receptive fields, often selective for orientation of a visual stimulus.

(Macaque)

Cytochrome Oxidase stain V1 blobs -> V2 this stripes V1 ‘interblobs’ (I.e., not blobs) -> V2 inter- stripes V1 layer 4B -> V2 thick stripes

Another view – note ‘illusory’ border from offset lines in D.

V4 -> One of the larger and more important of the post-V1/V2 visual cortical areas -> More of a ‘parvo’ sort of pathway. ‘Ventral Stream’ -> More complex responsivities to shapes than just oriented lines (real or imagined). Larger RF size. -> Complex color properties (used to be thought of as the color center, now know that it does more than just color) -> Attentional effects become easier to elicit -> Lesions cause more subtle problems than just visual field defects.

V4 has larger RF’s – here we can fit two stimuli inside. This is from experimental data from a monkey recording from a single V4 neuron. If the monkey is attending to a location where an effective stimulus (I.e., one that elicits a strong response) is present, you get a strong response (left). However, if the monkey is not paying attention to that location, you get a much weaker response, even though the optical stimulus is identical (right).

As before but now the monkey is only paying or not paying attention to the entire receptive field. There are lots of different variations of this basic paradigm. Very much harder (though not impossible) to see such effects in V1/V2.

V4 neurons often have more complex shape selectivities

V4 neurons often have more complex color properties Our perception of color is NOT a simple function of the amount of red, green, and blue light. (Ever take a picture using daylight film and incandescent light?). A V4 neuron responding to a red square in the middle of a lot of different color patches (‘Mondrian’). It responds strongest when the rest of the scene is lit by white light (RGB), weakest when everything is lit by red (R) thus making color distinctions impossible.

In experimental animals lesioning V4 does not result in specific visual field defects, but in more generalized problems of shape perception. For example, an animal with a V4 lesion may be able to learn to recognize an object in one orientation, but be unable to recognize it if it is viewed at a different angle or distance. Also, lesions of V4 have been associated with problems in color vision.

IT -> Inferior temporal cortex (IT, TE, TEO, etc.) -> Last of the hierarchy of (more-or-less) purely visual areas in the ventral (‘parvo’, ‘what’) stream. -> Neurons can have very large receptive fields… -> …but at the same time, the specificity for visual stimuli can be very high -> Lesions of IT can have devastating consequences for the ability to recognize specific objects (e.g. faces) with no corresponding loss of acuity or visual field deficits. -> Large attentional effects -> Lesions of temporal cortex can cause visual field deficits by interrupting the passing fibers of Myer’s loops.

From Van Essen and Colleagues

RF sizes get larger further along in the looser hierarchy of visual areas… (And by IT they often cross the midline)

IT neurons in the same cortical column tend to have similar (not identical) properties

Remember the columns in V1? It has been speculated that there is a similar modular organization in IT, but if so, we do not know what the analogs of location in space, orientation, or ocular dominance are.

“Grandmother Cell” Hypothesis IT cells can be so specific in their response properties that it seems like a neuron might only fire when one specific object (like someone’s grandmother) comes into view. Thus, if the neuron that is ‘tuned’ to your grandmother fires, that is a signal to the rest of the brain that grandma is there. However, while IT neurons have very specific response properties, they still do respond to more than just one object. Current thinking is that the presence of a particular object (face) is coded for by a pattern of of activity of a relatively small (but still greater than one) number of IT cells. Thus, at least for now the ‘grandmother cell hypothesis’ is no longer generally accepted.

MT/V5 -> Middle Temporal cortex (MT, but only really middle temporal in owl monkeys!) related areas are Mst, and various parietal something-or-other). -> In the hierarchy of (more-or-less) purely visual areas in the dorsal (‘magno’, ‘where’) stream. -> Specialized for motion processing -> MT is not large but it appears to be important and it has been studied a lot, so it gets a lot of coverage in textbooks. -> Lots of heavily myelinated axons, fast conduction velocities (a magno trait). Stains heavily for myelin compared to other visual cortical areas. -> Cells in MT are really easy to activate with moving stimuli.

Analysis of Motion As usual, what seems easy is really incredibly hard. Figuring out what is going where in a visual scene is a very hard computational task – one that we are not even close to solving for computer vision. For starters, we sense motion when our eyes are fixed and an image moves across the retina, and equally when the eyes track a moving object and the image on the retina is fixed.

The visual system can also infer motion from isolated sightings of an object in different locations at different times: Apparent Motion.

The visual system can also solve the Aperture Problem.

Unlike neurons in V1, some (not all!) neurons in MT appear to solve the aperture problem: they respond to the integrated motion of the entire object, not just to the motion of its separate parts.

MST: Medial Superior Temporal area -> Gets most of its inputs from MT -> Neurons are tuned to flow fields.

Patterns of optic flow

The parietal lobe is in many ways the least well understood of the cortical lobes. Contains primary somatosensory cortex, and a lot of regions dealing with vision. Important for hand-eye coordination and regulating attention, possibly creating a ‘sense of space’. Lesions can cause hemi-neglect (“eyes right). Some areas found in monkey posterior parietal cortex: LIP: Lateral Intraparietal Cortex. Contains a retinotopic map of salience. Neurons respond well to recent stimuli, or to stimuli that are behaviorally relevant. Predictive remapping across saccades! VIP: Ventral Intraparietal Cortex: A multisensory map of space around the mouth and face, important for ingestive behaviors?

Other cues to relative size and motion have to be evaluated by the visual system, and integrated with all the other sources of information.

Binocular Disparity: Judging distance by triangulating between the two eyes. First neurons sensitive to binocular disparity are found in area V1. Disparity sensitive neurons are found throughout visual cortex, but are most easily and commonly found in the dorsal/parietal pathway. Near disparity = crossed Far disparity = uncrossed

Single-Image Random-Dot Stereogram! Autostereogram Random-Dot Autosterogram “Magic Eye”

So how do they work? Standard stereograms use two images, one for each eye. Various optical systems ensure that the left eye gets one image, and the right eye gets another image. For autostereograms, the bottom line is that there are two hidden pictures in a single image seen by both eyes, the two hidden images have different amounts of horizontal shift in different regions of the image that are interpreted by the brain as different depths. Using random dots hides the two images well, because as with standard random dot stereograms, you can’t see the images unless they have been binocularly fused.

Actually looking at two objects, one nearer and one farther than the fixation point. The brain interprets the shifts in the image of the objects as evidence of them being nearer or farther than fixation.

Standard stereogram, each eye gets a separate image. As before, the brain interprets the shifts in the image of the objects between the two retinas as evidence of them being nearer or farther than fixation.

Single image stereogram, two shifted copies of each object, the brain again interprets the shifts in the image of the objects between the two retinas as evidence of them being nearer or farther than fixation. BUT: How do you hide the two images?

Very crudely: use random-dot stereograms. You can’t see the objects unless they are binocularly fused, thus hiding the two pictures. At each place in the single image, you have two overlaid (transparent or alternating) random dot stereograms with different horizontal shifts. The brain uses the shift that produces the best match to estiamte distance. Why does it take time to see these images? Because in normal conditions there are false binocular matches everywhere, and the visual system ignores them unless there is confirmatory data. Initially all the little random dot stereogram patches are filtered out. Once a few manage to break through at once and give the sense of a colinera contour, this reinforces the depth sense from the random dot stereograms and the entire picture snaps into focus.

Tricks for viewing: Most autostereograms are designed for wall-eyed (diverging) viewing. Need to focus (converge) on a point behind the image plane. People with stereo vision deficits can’t fuse random-dot stereograms, including magic eye (between 1 and 5 percent of population, give or take). -> Use bright lighting: causes pupil to constrict, decreases conflicting information due to blur and depth-of-focus. -> Try to look behind the image. Look at your reflection if on glossy paper (which optically will appear twice as far away). Hold up to your nose and slowly pull away. Look at an object behind the image plane (opposite if autostereogram designed for crossed viewing).