Highly Fluent, Balanced Bilingualism Does Not Enhance Executive Function Oliver Sawi 1,2, Jack Darrow 1, Hunter Johnson 1, Kenneth Paap 1 ; 1 San Francisco.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Encouraging enterprise Moving towards a zero-waste society Developing a capable population Fostering resilient communities Advancing global citizenship.
Advertisements

CRELLA University of Bedfordshire May 2012 Parvaneh Tavakoli Effects of Task Design on Native and Non-native Task Performance.
{ “Age” Effects on Second Language Acquisition Examination of 4 hypotheses related to age and language learning
 Residents report using different modes of communication with LEP patients depending on the clinical encounter.  Variation in professional interpreter.
QUESTIONNAIRES ORANGE BOOK CHAPTER 9. WHAT DO QUESTIONNAIRES GATHER? BEHAVIOR ATTITUDES/BELIEFS/OPINIONS CHARACTERISTICS (AGE / MARITAL STATUS / EDUCATION.
Method Participants Fifty-six undergraduate students (age range 19-37), 14 in each of the four language groups (monolingual, Spanish-English bilingual,
LANE 424 Seminar in Linguistics
Experiment 2: MEG Study Materials and Methods: 11 right-handed subjects with 20:20 vision were run. 3 subjects’ data was discarded because of poor performance.
Understanding the Bilingual Education Controversy Let us remember: Goals of Bilingual Education: Subject matter knowledge, i.e., the development of academic.
Putting Together the Pieces: Meaning Matters in Children’s Plural Comprehension Craig Van Pay, Areanna Lakowske & Jennifer Zapf.
A multilingual brain is a smarter brain: recent findings from neuroscience and implications for education Dara Ghahremani, PhD Department of Psychiatry,
Effects of Bilingualism and Task Switching on Hemispheric Interaction Suzanne E. Welcome & Christine Chiarello University of California, Riverside Introduction.
Language Hull & Vaid, 2006 Pre-IB.
Effects of Bilingualism on the A ttention N etwork T est: I ts significance and Implications Sujin Yang & Barbara Lust Cornell University (
1 Examining the role of Self-Regulated Learning on Introductory Programming Performance Susan Bergin, Ronan Reilly and Des Traynor Department of Computer.
Sex Differences in Visual Field Lateralization: Where are they? Christine Chiarello 1, Laura K. Halderman 1, Suzanne Welcome 1, Janelle Julagay 1 & Christiana.
Language Proficiency and Executive Control in Bilingual Children with TLD and with SLI Peri Iluz-Cohen Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel.
Influence of Word Class Proportion on Cerebral Asymmetries for High and Low Imagery Words Christine Chiarello 1, Connie Shears 2, Stella Liu 3, and Natalie.
Effects of Bilingualism on Hemispheric Interaction Suzanne E. Welcome & Christine Chiarello University of California, Riverside Maintaining and coordinating.
Chapter 7 Correlational Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian
SPSS Session 4: Association and Prediction Using Correlation and Regression.
Relationships Among Variables
Statistical Analyses & Threats to Validity
Test-Retest Reliability and Convergent Validity of Measures of Executive Function Oliver Sawi & Kenneth Paap Department of Psychology, San Francisco State.
Measuring Individual Components of Executive Processing Oliver Sawi, Zachary Greenberg, & Kenneth Paap Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University,
Results Following Signal Detection Theory, Accuracy is calculated as the difference between Real and Foil claim rates, and Bias is the mean of the two.
Evaluating the Validity of NLSC Self-Assessment Scores Charles W. Stansfield Jing Gao Bill Rivers.
Near East University Department of English Language Teaching Advanced Research Techniques Correlational Studies Abdalmonam H. Elkorbow.
The Dynamic Analog Scale: Using a Single Item to Measure Personality INTRODUCTION When measuring personality traits, personality psychologists typically.
Counseling Research: Quantitative, Qualitative, and Mixed Methods, 1e © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. Basic Statistical Concepts Sang.
Illustration of a Validity Argument for Two Alternate Assessment Approaches Presentation at the OSEP Project Directors’ Conference Steve Ferrara American.
The Impact of Exposure to MSA on the Acquisition of Basic Language and Literacy Skills in Arabic Elinor Saiegh-Haddad Bar-Ilan University
The Influence of Feature Type, Feature Structure and Psycholinguistic Parameters on the Naming Performance of Semantic Dementia and Alzheimer’s Patients.
Age of Acquisition and Cognitive Advantage in Bilingualism Willie Lin and Vivian Cardona.
AN ANALYSIS OF THE EFFECTIVENESS OF THE EARLY READING INTERVENTION FOR SELF-EFFICACY (E-RISE) ON FIRST, SECOND, AND THIRD GRADE STUDENTS IN AN AT-RISK.
Results Introduction Nonconditional Feedback Selectively Eliminates Conflict Adaption Summary Methods 38 participants performed a parity judgment task.
Educational Research Chapter 13 Inferential Statistics Gay, Mills, and Airasian 10 th Edition.
The Source of Enhanced Cognitive Control in Bilinguals: Evidence From Bimodal-Bilinguals Gigi Luk 1, Jennie Pyers 2, Karen Emmorey 3 & Ellen Bialystok.
20,000 Flanker Trials: Are the Effects Reliable, Robust, and Stable? Ken Paap 1, Sarah Wagner 1, Hunter Johnson 1,Morgan Bockelman 1, Donish Cushing 1,
Icebreaker WWhich state has the most bilingual and multilingual residents? A. New Mexico B. New York C. Texas D. California.
The Role of Mixed Emotional States in Predicting Men’s and Women’s Subjective and Physiological Sexual Responses to Erotic Stimuli Peterson, Z. D. 1 and.
Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Nonsocial Events Across Infancy: Effects of Unimodal versus Bimodal Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
The Source of Enhanced Cognitive Control in Bilinguals: Evidence From Bimodal-Bilinguals Gigi Luk 1, Jennie Pyers 2, Karen Emmorey 3 & Ellen Bialystok.
Chapter 6 - Standardized Measurement and Assessment
Dyadic Patterns of Parental Perceptions of Health- Related Quality of Life Gustavo R. Medrano & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pediatric.
PET Count  Word Frequency effects (coefficients) were reliably related to activation in both the striate and ITG for older adults only.  For older adults,
Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning: Dual Task Studies Claire Conway, Rebecca Bull & Louise Phillips School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen,
General and Feeding Specific Behavior Problems in a Community Sample of Children Amy J. Majewski, Kathryn S. Holman & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.
“ Language Maintenance versus Language of Instruction: Spanish Reading Development among Latino and Latina Bilingual Learners” Authors: C. Patrick Proctor,
Thirty Country Experts and Core Scientific Research Team European Union Commisioned.
IMPACTS OF SERVICE DELIVERY ON SLD IDENTIFICATION, TEACHER EMPLOYMENT, AND OUTCOMES Dr. Paul Sindelar Christopher Leko University of Florida.
Assistant Instructor Nian K. Ghafoor Feb Definition of Proposal Proposal is a plan for master’s thesis or doctoral dissertation which provides the.
Working memory and second language learning Graham Hitch, Meesha Warmington & Swathi Kandru.
Language Switching Frequency in Bilinguals is Inconsistently Linked to Executive Functioning Hunter Johnson 1, Oliver Sawi 1,2, Kenneth Paap 1 ; 1 San.
●In a previous study, we used the Moral Foundations Theory approach (described in Graham, et al., 2011) to examine the moral differences between Christians,
Norming Study Mechanisms of Emotion Regulation: The Role of Attentional Control Lindsey R. Wallace, M. A. & Elisabeth J. Ploran, Ph.D. Department of Psychology,
Cognitive Processes in SLL and Bilinguals:
Working-memory: is there a bilingual advantage?
Hunter Johnson1, Jack Darrow1, Oliver Sawi1,2, Kenneth Paap1;
BACKGROUND Experiment Current study Results Discussion Elevator Task
Kelly A. Vaughn & Arturo E. Hernandez University of Houston
Presentation by Hanh Dinh and Beverly Beaudette
Oliver Sawi1,2, Hunter Johnson1, Kenneth Paap1;
Linguistic Predictors of Cultural Identification in Bilinguals
Past and Present: Verb Tenses Across Blog Topics
Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind & Brain
Prosocial Behaviors in Adolescence
Multiple Linguistic Competencies
A cognitive perspective on cross language influence
Neuroimaging Impaired Response Inhibition and Salience Attribution in Human Drug Addiction: A Systematic Review  Anna Zilverstand, Anna S. Huang, Nelly.
Presentation transcript:

Highly Fluent, Balanced Bilingualism Does Not Enhance Executive Function Oliver Sawi 1,2, Jack Darrow 1, Hunter Johnson 1, Kenneth Paap 1 ; 1 San Francisco State University, 2 University of Connecticut Executive functions (EFs) consist of a set of general-purpose control processes believed to be central to the self-regulation of thoughts and behaviors that are instrumental to accomplishing goals. EFs include components for inhibitory control, switching, monitoring, and updating. Method Introduction Results Executive Function References to our Earlier Work Tables and Figures Bilinguals have two lexicons that remain active even when the context strongly supports the intention to use only one of them (e.g., Brysbaert, 1998), necessitating a mechanism for maintaining separation between languages. If this mechanism (Green 1998) involves the same executive functions used in all domains, then bilinguals accrue massive amounts of practice that should generalize across cognitive domains and make them less vulnerable to interference in nonlinguistic tasks. The Bilingual Advantage Hypothesis Two Perspectives Relating Degree of Balance to Bilingual Advantages Perspective 1: Enhanced EF is directly related to the use of and proficiency acquired in L2. Highly proficient and balanced bilinguals should be best and monolinguals with little or no exposure to an L2 should be worst. From this perspective some of Paap & Greenberg’s bilinguals may not have been bilingual enough. Perspective 2: High levels of use and proficiency in both L1 and L2 lead to highly practiced and automatic language control mechanism. Thus, bilinguals with dominant L1’s rely more on cognitive control and this should lead to the greatest levels of EF enhancement. From this perspective some of Paap & Greenberg’s monolinguals may have been too bilingual. Tasks & Participants: 384 participants completed two to four of the following tasks: antisaccade, flanker, Simon, and color-shape switching. The four tasks are described in Paap and Greenberg (2013). Only Active bilinguals: 23 bilinguals who reported currently using English more than 94 percent of the time were removed Language switching: The median and modal scale value for switching was 3: “I usually switch from one language to the other a couple of times a day.” Proficiency: Listening and speaking skills were self-rated using this scale: 7 (Super Fluency, better than a typical native speaker); 6 (Fluent, as good as a typical native speaker); 5 (Near Fluency); 4 (Advanced Intermediate); 3 (Intermediate); 2 (Advanced Beginner); 1 (Beginner) and 0 (no exposure to an L2). Balance Groups: As shown in Table 1, Five groups were formed based on the L1/L2 ratio. The Hi Pro group is highly proficient (a 6 or 7) in both languages. Each successive group pairs a highly proficient L1 with a lower proficiency in L2. Paap, K. R. (2014). The role of componential analysis, categorical hypothesising, replicability and confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages in executive functioning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, Paap, K. R. & Liu, Y. (2014). Conflict resolution in sentence processing is the same for bilinguals and monolinguals: The role of confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 27(1), Sawi, O., & Paap, K. (2013, April). Test–retest reliability and convergent validity of measures of executive processing: evidence from the Simon, flanker, switching, and antisaccade task. Poster presented at the meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, CA. There were no group differences in the magnitude of the flanker effect, switching costs, mixing costs, antisaccade costs (percent correct or RT) or global RT in either the Simon or flanker tasks. Thus, seven measures of EF show no support for a bilingual advantage in any bilingual group. However, the main effect of group on the Simon effect was significant, F(4, 374) = 3.29, p =.011. The highest proficiency group had the largest interference effect and the monolingual group- with no exposure to an L2 had the smallest. Not only is this a bilingual disadvantage, but the pattern is completely opposite the predictions of Perspective 1 that the high proficiency groups should be the best and the “pure” monolinguals the worst. Although the low proficiency bilinguals had smaller Simon effects than the high proficiency bilinguals this does not support Perspective 2 because the pure monolinguals had even smaller Simon effects. Differences in EF across groups based on relative proficiency of L1 to L2 Regression Analyses on Bilinguals Stepwise regression analyses were conducted on 10 different measures of EF in our database using the following four predictors: age-of-acquisition of L2, balance, percentage of English use, and parent’s educational level (PED). Only participants classified as bilingual (L1 and L2 proficiency ≥ 4) were included in these analyses. Only 3 of the 10 models were significant and each of those consisted of a single predictor. The regression model predicting the magnitude of the Simon effect was significant and balance was the only significant predictor; standardized β = +.19, t(161) = 2.45, p =.015. The positive β coefficient means that as the degree of balance increases the magnitude of the Simon effect increases. Thus, managing two languages with high proficiency is actually associated with larger interference effects. The regression analysis of the Simon effect is consistent with the group analysis. The other two significant models have age-of-acquisition as the sole significant predictor with one measure (mixing costs in the switching task) showing better EF scores for early bilinguals and the other (flanker interference effect) showing the opposite. Does Managing Three Languages Enhance EF? Yet another hypothesis is that having to manage three languages provides the greatest enhancement of EF. A separate independent groups (monolingual, bilingual, trilingual) ANOVA was performed on each of the measures of EF used in the previous analyses. Our composite database has 35 trilinguals with mean L1, L2, and L3 proficiencies of 6.7, 5.9, and 4.7, respectively. The main effect of group was not significant for 9 of the 10 measures, all p’s >.29. The magnitude of the Simon effect did significantly differ across groups, F(2, 376) = 3.85, p =.022; but as shown in the figure to the left there is a monolingual advantage (smaller Simon effect) over both bilinguals and trilinguals. Conclusion The highly proficient and balanced bilinguals in our composite database do not outperform other groups in the antisaccade, flanker, Simon, and switching tasks. Neither do trilinguals. These results complement those of Duñabeitia et al. (2014) using Basque-Spanish bilinguals and Gathercole et al. (2014) using Welsh-English bilinguals showing no advantages for bilinguals who are highly proficient, acquire both languages early, and reside in language communities where most people speak the same two languages and switching is ubiquitous. Furthermore these studies use a very large N, use multiple measures of EF, and test across a wide age range.