Speech Dynamics The Main Idea: At an abstract linguistic level, phonetic segments ([b], [p], [r], [k], [i], [ ɑ ], [u], etc.) are discrete, independent,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Grammar: Meaning and Contexts * From Presentation at NCTE annual conference in Pittsburgh, 2005.
Advertisements

SPPA 403 Speech Science1 Unit 3 outline The Vocal Tract (VT) Source-Filter Theory of Speech Production Capturing Speech Dynamics The Vowels The Diphthongs.
The sound patterns of language
Phonology, part 5: Features and Phonotactics
The Sound Patterns of Language: Phonology
Phonology Phonology is essentially the description of the systems and patterns of speech sounds in a language. It is, in effect, based on a theory of.
MORPHOLOGY - morphemes are the building blocks that make up words.
Speech Production Process
Static Vowels ► Diphthongs ► Semivowels ► Stops Extending SF Theory to Accommodate Articulatory Movement.
Introduction to Linguistics and Basic Terms
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Representing language.
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 2: Language processing: speed and flexibility.
What is Phonetics? Short answer: The study of speech sounds in all their aspects. Phonetics is about describing speech. (Note: phonetics ¹ phonics) Phonetic.
Linguisitics Levels of description. Speech and language Language as communication Speech vs. text –Speech primary –Text is derived –Text is not “written.
Chapter three Phonology
1 ENGLISH PHONETICS AND PHONOLOGY Lesson 3A Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology.
Neuron Note #1 AP Psychology review.
Syntax.
Lecture 1 Introduction: Linguistic Theory and Theories
Generative Grammar(Part ii)
Spelling Lists.
Phonetics October 10, 2012 Housekeeping Morphology homeworks are due!
Linguistic Theory Lecture 3 Movement. A brief history of movement Movements as ‘special rules’ proposed to capture facts that phrase structure rules cannot.
Style, Grammar and Punctuation
Spelling Lists. Unit 1 Spelling List write family there yet would draw become grow try really ago almost always course less than words study then learned.
Phonetics October 8, 2010 Housekeeping Morphology homeworks are due! Also: I will be gone next week… Danica will be taking over the reins.
CSD 2230 HUMAN COMMUNICATION DISORDERS
English Pronunciation Practice A Practical Course for Students of English By Wang Guizhen Faculty of English Language & Culture Guangdong University of.
Phonetics HSSP Week 5.
By Lecturer Samarnh Phonology and Phonetics. Three components of language SoundsStructuresMeanings Phonetics (Smallest unit of sound), e.g. /b/ Morphology.
Substance Substance & Form Diachronic and Synchronic approaches Substance & Form Diachrony& Synchrony Lecture # 12.
Speech & Language Development 1 Normal Development of Speech & Language Language...“Standardized set of symbols and the knowledge about how to combine.
Phonetics and Phonology
Phonology, part 4: Distinctive Features
1 Speech Perception 3/30/00. 2 Speech Perception How do we perceive speech? –Multifaceted process –Not fully understood –Models & theories attempt to.
Speech Science Fall 2009 Nov 2, Outline Suprasegmental features of speech Stress Intonation Duration and Juncture Role of feedback in speech production.
Language. Language Communication – transmitting information Many animals communicate Call systems – system of communication limited to a set number of.
1. Information Conveyed by Speech 2. How Speech Fits in with the Overall Structure of Language TWO TOPICS.
Phonology The sound patterns of language Nuha Alwadaani March, 2014.
Voice and Diction Chapter 3.
Speech Science IX How is articulation organized? Version WS
A problem with linguistic explanations  A problem with linguistic explanations  Controlling articulatory movements  Memory for speech  The balance.
Introduction to Linguistics Ms. Suha Jawabreh Lecture # 8.
Language. Phonetics is the study of how elements of language are physically produced.
Levels of Language 6 Levels of Language. Levels of Language Aspect of language are often referred to as 'language levels'. To look carefully at language.
Phonetics February 7, 2012 Housekeeping Morphology homeworks are due!
Chapter 3 Culture and Language. Chapter Outline  Humanity and Language  Five Properties of Language  How Language Works  Language and Culture  Social.
1 Prof.Roseline WEEK-4 LECTURE -4 SYNTAX. 2 Prof.Roseline Syntax Concentrate on the structure and ordering of components within a sentence Greater focus.
Phonology, part 4: Natural Classes and Features November 2, 2012.
What is phonetics? Phonetics is the scientific study of speech sounds. It consists of three main sub-fields:  Articulatory phonetics  = how speech sounds.
Sounds and speech perception Productivity of language Speech sounds Speech perception Integration of information.
Understanding English Variation Connected Speech Processes What are connected speech processes? Connected speech processes are changes in the pronunciation.
Rules, Movement, Ambiguity
Chapter II phonology II. Classification of English speech sounds Vowels and Consonants The basic difference between these two classes is that in the production.
Chapter Five Language Description language study and linguistic study 1Applied Linguistics Chapter 5 by TIAN Bing.
Introduction to Language Phonetics 1. Explore the relationship between sound and spelling Become familiar with International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA )
1 Syntax 1. 2 In your free time Look at the diagram again, and try to understand it. Phonetics Phonology Sounds of language Linguistics Grammar MorphologySyntax.
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
3 Phonology: Speech Sounds as a System No language has all the speech sounds possible in human languages; each language contains a selection of the possible.
Speech Production “Problems” Key problems that science must address How is speech coded? How is speech coded? What is the size of the “basic units” of.
Against formal phonology (Port and Leary).  Generative phonology assumes:  Units (phones) are discrete (not continuous, not variable)  Phonetic space.
Chapter 1 Introduction PHONOLOGY (Lane 335). Phonetics & Phonology Phonetics: deals with speech sounds, how they are made (articulatory phonetics), how.
Introduction to Linguistics
Static Vowels ► Diphthongs ► Semivowels ► Stops
Part I: Basics and Constituency
Speech Dynamics The Main Idea: At an abstract linguistic level, speech sounds ([b], [p], [r], [k], [i], [ɑ], [u], etc.) are discrete, independent, interchangeable.
English Phonetics and Phonology
What is Phonetics? Short answer: The study of speech sounds in all their aspects. Phonetics is about describing speech. (Note: phonetics ¹ phonics) Phonetic.
Natural Language Processing
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
Presentation transcript:

Speech Dynamics The Main Idea: At an abstract linguistic level, phonetic segments ([b], [p], [r], [k], [i], [ ɑ ], [u], etc.) are discrete, independent, interchangeable snap-together parts – like beads on a string. The  bead is snapped out, an [ ɪ ] bead is snapped in, changing [kæt] into [k ɪ t]. The [t]  bead is snapped out, an [n] bead is snapped in, changing [k ɪ t] into [k ɪ n]. The [ ɪ ] bead is snapped out, an [æ] bead is snapped in, changing [k ɪ n] into [kæn]. The [k] bead is snapped out, an [m] bead is snapped in, changing [kæn] into [mæn . The word  is built by stringing together 3 distinct, discrete “beads” – [k], [æ], and [t]. Artist’s rendition of beads.

The phonetic structure of speech is an example of a discrete combinatorial system: morphemes and words are built by combining separate, independent, snap-together parts called phonemes or phonetic segments. DNA works this way as well: Genes are built by combining a finite (and very small) number of discrete, snap-together parts (adenine, guanine, cytosine, & thymine) in an infinite variety of combinations.

The letters that are used in any alphabetic writing system comprise a discrete combinatorial system – you get infinite variety by combining 26 discrete elements – a, b, c, d … z. Are there aspects of language other than morphemes & words built by combining phonemes that comprise a discrete combinatorial system? (Answer: Yes. Language is a discrete combinatorial system everywhere you look. Words are constructed by using morphological rules to combine discrete parts called morphemes. Phrases are constructed by using syntactic and semantic rules to combine words – nouns, verbs, articles, prepositions, etc. Sentences are constructed by using syntactic rules to combine phrases, the simplest being S  NP + VP; i.e., a sentence is composed of a noun phrase and a verb phrase. So, language is a discrete combinatorial system at all levels.)

The snap-together parts in a discrete combinatorial system are: (1) discrete, (2) independent. (1) discrete = digital rather than analog (continuous); e.g., in genetics, the bases must be one of four discrete choices: A, G, C, or T; no such thing as guanine and 3/4 th ; adenine and a 1/2. In phonetics, at an abstract level, snap-together phonemes are one of ~40 discrete choices; e.g., /b/ or /p/, not /b/ and 2/3 rd. (2) independent = the snap-together parts do not affect one another; e.g., in genetics, guanine is guanine whether it is attached to adenine, thymine, cytosine, or another guanine. In phonemics (not phonetics), /p/ is /p/, whether the next phoneme is / ɑ / or /i/ or /æ/ or /r/ or /l/ or whatever.

OK, now the big deal about accommodation, coarticulation, and assimilation. Here’s the idea we started with: At an abstract linguistic level, phonetic segments are discrete, independent, interchangeable snap-together parts. The key phrase here is at an abstract linguistic level. At an abstract linguistic level phonemes are discrete, independent, and interchangeable snap- together parts.

However, at the actual level of speech production – real movements of real articulators generating real speech sounds – phonetic segments are: not discrete not independent not interchangeable

    In a world of discrete, independent, interchangeable parts, the two instances of [s] would be identical. Are they? The phonetic environment in which a sound occurs can have a strong influence on the way a sound is produced. This is an example of coarticulation – the lip rounding from the [u] carries over to the [s]; i.e., the rounded lips for [u] just stay rounded for [s]. spectra measured during the 

This shows what happens when the [s] of [isi] is cut and pasted to [usu], and vice versa. Do the cross-spliced syllables sound ok? What does this tell us about the interchange- ability of the phonetic elements that are being recombined in this discrete combinatorial system? [Answer: They aren’t]  from   from   [  ]

The phenomenon of coarticulation is nothing more than an articulatory shortcut. Without coarticulation, producing  would involve: 1)round the lips (and adjust the tongue) for [u] 2)retract the lips (and adjust the tongue) for [s] 3)round the lips again (and adjust the tongue) for [u] It’s not that this can’t be done. It can. Easily. The problem is that it slows you down. A lot. Most people speak almost as fast as they can almost all the time. And we talk fast – ~10 speech sounds per second and up.

In terms of the # of muscles, the complexity of the movements, and the precision of the movements, there is nothing we do that approaches speech for speed, complexity, and efficiency. The primary reason for this: coarticulation. Coarticulation is not an arcane, egghead detail. It’s the single most important fact about speech motor control.

If you understand what’s going on in this one example – [isi] vs. [usu] – then you understand most of what you need to know about coarticulation. Basic Motor-Planning Principle: The motor planning system will take any shortcut that the ear and the rules of the language will tolerate. That’s it. (Memorize that sentence, and be sure you understand what it means. No kidding.) [isi] vs. [usu]: If the lip-rounding shortcut produced a fricative that was so distorted that it no longer sounded like an [s], the shortcut wouldn’t be used. But the [s] remains quite intelligible, so the shortcut is used.

Another example (we’ve already seen this one): geese vs. gone ([gis] vs. [g ɔ n]) Is the place of articulation the same? [Hint: No. Place for [g ɔ n] is velar, place for [gis] is palatal.] What’s going on here; i.e., what’s the shortcut? Why is the place of articulation further forward for [gis] than [g ɔ n]? [Answer: You get a more forward place of artic for the front vowel than for the back vowel, so the movements take less time.] Would this shortcut work for a language that has both velar and palatal stops? [Answer: Nah] This is, in part, what was meant earlier by “and the rules of the language.”

Another example we’ve seen: bat vs. man ([bæt] vs. [mæn]) What happens to the velum during the [æ] in these two words? Phonetically, this is: [bæt] vs. [mæ̃n] What’s the shortcut? Why is the shortcut tolerated? [A: Listeners are ok with nasalized vowels]

Assimilation There are times when the shortcut is not tolerated by the ear directly, but it’s allowed by still allowed by listeners. These are examples of assimilation. Transcribe these: this steak vs. this shoe What’s the shortcut? Do you get an acceptable [s] in the word “this” in both cases? Produce this sentence as naturally as you can: I get my news by listening to NPR. How about this: I called her from a phone booth. And this, naturally and at a fairly rapid speaking rate: did he vs. did you

Terminology Accommodation: MacKay’s word for an articulatory shortcut. Two Varieties of Accommodation Coarticulation The sound that’s modified by the shortcut is directly tolerated by the ear; e.g., the [s] in [usu] still sounds like an [s]; the [g] in [gis] still sounds like a [g], etc. Assimilation The sound that’s modified by the shortcut is not directly tolerated by the ear; e.g., the underlying /s/ of “this” in “this shoe” no longer sounds like an /s/; the modified sound – now an [ ʃ ] – is tolerated by the language system.

Last point: The impression you may have is that the motor planning system cares only about speed. This can’t be. Imagine a speaker who says only this: “buh-buh-buh” – very fast, but you can't distinguish one word from another. Mature speakers do not pronounce cupcake as cupake [k ʌ pek], though that would be faster; or cukake [k ʌ kek] ( toddlers take that kind of shortcut all the time – but NOT for reasons of speed ), though that too would be faster. Why?

The motor planning system is some poorly understood compromise among three factors: (1) speed (2) intelligibility (3) what listeners will accept as natural The last one is the trickiest. For reasons that are not too clear, listeners are ok with thish shoe [ð ɪʃ u] and foam booth [fom buθ]) but they’re not ok with cukake [k ʌ kek] or “cupake” [k ʌ pek].

Summary Details aside, here’s the key idea: The basic motor-planning principle underlying accommodation (coarticulation & assimilation) is this: The motor planning system will take any shortcut that listeners will tolerate (i.e., the speech needs to be intelligible and it needs to sound natural). Q: Why do we take these shortcuts? A: Simple: They allow us to speak faster.