Pitch Perception.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Perception Chapter 11: Hearing and Listening
Advertisements

Hearing relative phases for two harmonic components D. Timothy Ives 1, H. Martin Reimann 2, Ralph van Dinther 1 and Roy D. Patterson 1 1. Introduction.
Frequency selectivity of the auditory system. Frequency selectivity Important for aspects of auditory perception such as, pitch, loudness, timbre, melody,
The Physics of Sound Sound begins with a vibration of an object Vibrating object transfers energy to air medium All complex vibration patterns seen as.
Timbre perception. Objective Timbre perception and the physical properties of the sound on which it depends Formal definition: ‘that attribute of auditory.
Periodicity and Pitch Importance of fine structure representation in hearing.
Physics 1251 The Science and Technology of Musical Sound Unit 1 Session 8 Harmonic Series Unit 1 Session 8 Harmonic Series.
Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a frequency analyzer Chris Darwin.
CS 551/651: Structure of Spoken Language Lecture 11: Overview of Sound Perception, Part II John-Paul Hosom Fall 2010.
Pitch organisation in Western tonal music. Pitch in two dimensions Pitch perception in music is often thought of in two dimensions, pitch height and pitch.
Lecture 8  Perceived pitch of a pure tone  Absolute pitch  Midterm review Instructor: David Kirkby
PITCH AND TIMBRE MUSICAL ACOUSTICS Science of Sound Chapter 7.
INTRODUCTION TO HEARING. WHAT IS SOUND? amplitude Intensity measured in decibels.
Sensation and Perception - audition.ppt © 2001 Laura Snodgrass, Ph.D.1 Audition Anatomy –outer ear –middle ear –inner ear Ascending auditory pathway –tonotopic.
Music Perception. Why music perception? 1. Found in all cultures - listening to music is a universal activity. 2. Interesting from a developmental point.
A.Diederich– International University Bremen – Sensation and Perception – Fall Frequency Analysis in the Cochlea and Auditory Nerve cont'd The Perception.
A.Diederich – International University Bremen – USC – MMM – Spring 2005 Scales Roederer, Chapter 5, pp. 171 – 181 Cook, Chapter 14, pp. 177 – 185 Cook,
A.Diederich– International University Bremen – USC – MMM – Spring 5 1 The Perception of Frequency cont'd.
All you have is a pair of instruments (basilar membranes) that measure air pressure fluctuations over time Localization.
A brief message from your TAs Tine Gulbrandsen Wahab Hanif.
Timbre (pronounced like: Tamber) pure tones are very rare a single note on a musical instrument is a superposition (i.e. several things one on top of.
The Science of Sound Chapter 8
Hearing & Deafness (4) Pitch Perception 1. Pitch of pure tones 2. Pitch of complex tones.
PH 105 Dr. Cecilia Vogel Lecture 13. OUTLINE  Timbre and graphs:  Time graph  Spectrum graph  Spectrogram  Envelope  scales  units  interval factors.
Structure and function
Spectral centroid 6 harmonics: f0 = 100Hz E.g. 1: Amplitudes: 6; 5.75; 4; 3.2; 2; 1 [(100*6)+(200*5.75)+(300*4)+(400*3.2)+(500*2 )+(600*1)] / = 265.6Hz.
What are harmonics? Superposition of two (or more) frequencies yields a complex wave with a fundamental frequency.
The Science of Sound Chapter 8
Chapter 6: The Human Ear and Voice
© 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Conceptual Physics 11 th Edition Chapter 21: MUSICAL SOUNDS Noise and Music Musical Sounds Pitch Sound Intensity and Loudness.
Harmonics, Timbre & The Frequency Domain
Chapter 12 Preview Objectives The Production of Sound Waves
Abstract We report comparisons between a model incorporating a bank of dual-resonance nonlinear (DRNL) filters and one incorporating a bank of linear gammatone.
EE Audio Signals and Systems Kevin D. Donohue Electrical and Computer Engineering University of Kentucky.
Beats and Tuning Pitch recognition Physics of Music PHY103.
COMBINATION TONES The Science of Sound Chapter 8 MUSICAL ACOUSTICS.
Chapter 12 Objectives Differentiate between the harmonic series of open and closed pipes. Calculate the harmonics of a vibrating string and of open and.
Audio Scene Analysis and Music Cognitive Elements of Music Listening
Exam 1 February 6 – 7 – 8 – 9 Moodle testing centre.
Harmonics. Introduction Harmonic is obtained by a method of playing stringed instruments - playing the instrument while holding a finger against the string,
Hearing: auditory coding mechanisms. Harmonics/ Fundamentals ● Recall: most tones are complex tones, consisting of multiple pure tones ● The lowest frequency.
Extracting Melody Lines from Complex Audio Jana Eggink Supervisor: Guy J. Brown University of Sheffield {j.eggink
© Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company Preview Objectives The Production of Sound Waves Frequency of Sound Waves The Doppler Effect Chapter 12.
SOUND & THE EAR. Anthony J Greene2 Sound and the Ear 1.Sound Waves A.Frequency: Pitch, Pure Tone. B.Intensity C.Complex Waves and Harmonic Frequencies.
MUSICAL ACOUSTICS PITCH AND TIMBRE Science of Sound Chapter 7.
Hearing: Physiology and Psychoacoustics 9. The Function of Hearing The basics Nature of sound Anatomy and physiology of the auditory system How we perceive.
Harmonics. Each instrument has a mixture of harmonics at varying intensities Principle of superposition Periodics- Repeating patterns of waveforms.
Set 7 What’s that scale?? 1 Note Grades should be available on some computer somewhere. The numbers are based on the total number of correct answers,
Pitch Perception Or, what happens to the sound from the air outside your head to your brain….
Introduction to psycho-acoustics: Some basic auditory attributes For audio demonstrations, click on any loudspeaker icons you see....
Nuclear Accent Shape and the Perception of Syllable Pitch Rachael-Anne Knight LAGB 16 April 2003.
Pitch What is pitch? Pitch (as well as loudness) is a subjective characteristic of sound Some listeners even assign pitch differently depending upon whether.
The Overtone Series Derivation of Tonic Triad – Tonal Model Timbre
Sound Waves The production of sound from a sound wave begins with a vibrating object.
Applied Psychoacoustics Lecture 4: Pitch & Timbre Perception Jonas Braasch.
Chapter 12 Preview Objectives The Production of Sound Waves
Melinda Feldmann Combination Tones. What is a Combination Tone? Combination Tone In musical acoustics, faint tone produced in the inner ear by two simultaneously.
12-3 Harmonics.
Combination of tones (Road to discuss harmony) 1.Linear superposition If two driving forces are applied simultaneously, the response will be the sum of.
Physics Mrs. Dimler SOUND.  Every sound wave begins with a vibrating object, such as the vibrating prong of a tuning fork. Tuning fork and air molecules.
SPATIAL HEARING Ability to locate the direction of a sound. Ability to locate the direction of a sound. Localization: In free field Localization: In free.
HOW WE TRANSMIT SOUNDS? Media and communication 김경은 김다솜 고우.
PSYCHOACOUSTICS A branch of psychophysics
(Road to discuss harmony)
(Road to discuss harmony)
Pitch What is pitch? Pitch (as well as loudness) is a subjective characteristic of sound Some listeners even assign pitch differently depending upon whether.
Pitch What is pitch? Pitch (as well as loudness) is a subjective characteristic of sound Some listeners even assign pitch differently depending upon whether.
Pitch.
Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance
(Road to discuss harmony)
Presentation transcript:

Pitch Perception

Objective To understand the method (s) by which the auditory system processes a sound in order to determine its pitch. Audible range: 20 Hz – 20 kHz* The pitch of a sound refers to its perceived tonal height and is subjective; it requires the listener to make a perceptual judgement Variations in pitch create a sense of melody

Measuring pitch A method sometimes employed as an objective measure of assigning a pitch to a sound: the listener adjusts the frequency of a sound with a variable known frequency and similar timbre until the pitch of both sounds are perceived as being equal. This method gives the unit of Hertz (Hz) as a measure of the pitch frequency.

A complex sound is a sound containing more than one frequency component. The sound is harmonic if the frequency components occur at integer multiples of the frequency of a common (though not always present) fundamental component. The waveform of a harmonic sound repeats periodically at a rate equal to the frequency of the fundamental component.

Examples of harmonic sounds are the notes produced from musical instruments such as the violin, oboe and flute. Very clear sense of pitch Diagram: periodic waveform (upper) and spectrum (lower) of an oboe playing C4 Repetition rate of the waveform Fundamental component in the frequency spectrum

Pitch of a pure tone If the sound is a pure tone the perceived pitch generally corresponds to and varies with the frequency of the tone. Its pitch varies also to some extent with level (asa trk 28). What duration must a tone be, before it gives a sense of pitch (i.e. from ‘click’ to ‘tone’)? (asa trk 29) Pitch of a sound may be influenced by the presence of another sound (asa trk 30)

The pitch of harmonic sounds The pitch of a harmonic sound is found to vary mainly with changes in the fundamental frequency and so this is used as a measure of the pitch. Harmonic sounds - clearly defined sense of pitch Pitch of the ‘missing fundamental’ A pitch corresponding to the fundamental frequency may be perceived when it has been removed from the sound (asa trk 37)

Theories of Pitch Perception There are two important theories of how the auditory system is believed to code the pitch of a sound: the place theory and the temporal theory. The place theory is based on the fact that different frequency components of the input sound stimulate different places along the basilar membrane and in turn auditory nerve fibres with different characteristic frequencies.

Place theory of pitch perception The pitch of the sound is assumed to be related to the excitation pattern it produces on the BM. The pitch of a pure tone may be explained by the position of maximum excitation. For a sound made up of many frequency components, many different maxima occur along the basilar membrane at places corresponding to the frequencies of the components.

The position of the overall maximum, or the position of the maximum due to the lowest frequency component may not correspond to the perceived pitch of the sound. It is known that the pitch of a harmonic sound can remain the same even when energy at its fundamental frequency has been removed. This cannot be explained by the place theory

Temporal theory of pitch perception The waveform of a sound with a strong unambiguous pitch is periodic. The basis for the temporal theory of pitch perception is the timing of neural firings, which occur in response to vibrations on the basilar membrane. Nerve firings occur at particular phases of the waveform; a process called phase locking.

Temporal theory Due to phase locking the time intervals between the successive firings occur at approximately integer multiples of the period of the waveform. In this way the waveform periodicity that occurs at each place on the basilar membrane is coded. At some point in the auditory system these time intervals have to be measured.

Temporal theory The precision with which the nerve firings are linked to a particular phase in the waveform declines at high frequencies: upper limit of ~ 4-5 kHz. the ability to perceive pitches of sounds with fundamental frequencies greater than 5 kHz cannot be explained by this theory. It has been found that musical interval and melody perception decreases for sounds with fundamental frequencies greater than 5 kHz, although differences in frequency can still be heard.

Sounds produced by musical instruments, the human voice and most every day sounds have fundamental frequencies below 5 kHz.

In sum The auditory processing parts of the brain are supplied with information concerning the place of stimulation on the basilar membrane (place theory) and neural firing patterns (temporal theory). The importance of both types of information may depend on the frequencies present and the type of sound. Place coding may dominate for frequencies above 5 kHz where phase locking is reduced - below this temporal information may be dominant.

Frequency discrimination The ability to detect changes in frequency over time. difference limen for frequency (DLF) – the smallest detectable change in frequency For a pair of pure tones the listener judges whether the second tone is higher or lower in pitch than the first. The DLF is the frequency separation for which there is a certain percentage of correct responses, e.g. 75%. Frequency discrimination of pure tones (asa trk 33)

Frequency discrimination DLF varies from person to person The DLF has been found to depend on frequency, level, duration, suddenness of the frequency change and musical training A large increase in DLF for frequencies > 4-5 kHz DLF improves with increasing duration (at least up to 200ms, Moore 2003) DLF improves with increasing level.

Models of pitch perception Models to account for pitch perception in complex tones more than one frequency component, and hearing a pitch corresponding to the missing fundamental. Pattern recognition models – frequency analysis to determine individual components present, pattern recogniser to determine the pitch from the components

Pattern recognition models The pattern recogniser attempts to find the fundamental frequency that corresponds to the harmonics detected – template matching process. Some models of this type are: Goldstein, J. L., 1973. “An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones.” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 54(6), 1496-1516

Pattern recognition models Terhardt, E., G. Stoll and M. Seewann 1982. “Algorithm for the extraction of pitch and pitch salience from complex tonal signals.” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 71(3), 679-687 Terhardt E., G. Stoll and M. Seewann 1982. “Pitch of complex signals according to virtual-pitch theory: Tests examples and predictions.” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 71(3), 671

Model for pitch perception in complex tones Some models of pitch perception combine both place and temporal information Model proposed by Moore (2003) in earlier editions of his book

Bank of bandpass filters Neural transduction Analysis of spike intervals Combine intervals across CFs Most common interval Pitch

Bank of bandpass filters: - spectral analysis on the input sound filters organised according to their centre frequencies, representing tonotopic organisation of frequencies on the BM Each filter may be thought of as representing the frequency response of one point on the BM. bandwidths according to the ERB

Neural transduction – represents mechanical to neural transduction at the hair cell auditory nerve fibre synapse output stream of spike events precisely located in time to represent the signal produced by the auditory nerve fibres reflects the waveform structure produced at each point on the BM

Analysis of intervals – periodic output of filter channel – find period / frequencies present in each channel Compare time intervals across channels – for a harmonic sound the lowest common interval is that of the fundamental frequency Graph represents waveform at 4 points – first 4 harmonics for a 200 Hz fundamental

Harmonic Hz Period s 200 0.005 400 0.0025 600 0.00167 0.0033 800 0.00125 0.00375

Intervals between successive firings for a complex sound consisting of the frequency components 200, 400, 600 and 800 Hz. Intervals between successive nerve firings indicate the period of each individual harmonic, when sufficiently resolved. Lowest common time interval at 0.005s – corresponds to the 200 Hz fundamental frequency

Computer implementation of the above model: Meddis, R. and L. O’Mard. 1997. “A unitary model of pitch perception.” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 102 (3), 1811-1820

Pitch organisation in WTM The pitches of notes in WTM are most often tuned according to the system of equal temperament. Equal tempered tuning was formed out of a requirement for equally spaced intervals in terms of frequency ratio regardless of tonality (the musical key). Two notes are an octave apart if their frequencies are in the ratio 2:1. In equal tempered tuning the octave is divided into twelve equal logarithmic steps called semitones.

The fundamental frequency of adjacent semitones differs by a factor of 21/12. The semitone may be subdivided into ‘cents’. There are 100 cents in a semitone and therefore 1200 cents in an octave. The ratio of all equal tempered musical intervals (except for the unison and the octave) match approximately to the ratio of the corresponding pure tone interval in the harmonic series; the octave and the unsion match exactly

Only the octave and unison intervals can be described in terms of small integer ratios. All other intervals are tempered slightly from the small integer ratio (e.g. the fifth is tempered slightly less than pure, the fourth is slightly greater than pure and the major third is greater than pure).

Pitch in two dimensions Pitch perception in music is often thought of in two dimensions, pitch height and pitch chroma (Shepard, 1964). (asa trk 52) This is to account for the perceived similarity of pitches that are separated by octaves. Pitch height is the low / high dimension of pitch. The relative position of a pitch within a given octave is referred to as its chroma.

In Western music theory this is indicated by the octave equivalent pitch classes, C, C# etc. Music notes are identified first by their position within the octave, their chroma, and then by the octave in which they are placed (e.g. G3, F#6).