RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Harvest Data The total number of ducks harvested were 105 and 78, and the total number of shots taken were 403 and 265 using UV.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Role of Ultraviolet Light in Defense of Brood Parasitism by Dawna Lee-Olsen ECOL 484.
Advertisements

Puddle Ducks vs. Diving Ducks
Original Figures for "Molecular Classification of Cancer: Class Discovery and Class Prediction by Gene Expression Monitoring"
WATER DEPTH, VEGETATION, AND POLLUTANT REMOVAL IN A CONSTRUCTED WETLAND TREATING AQUACULTURE EFFLUENT Brian E. Dyson, Kim D. Jones, Ron Rosati* Department.
LEPIDOPTERA LARVAE AS AN INDICATOR OF MULTI-TROPHIC LEVEL RESPONSES TO CHANGING SEASONALITY IN THE ARCTIC K. M. Daly, 1 H. Steltzer, 1 L. Gough, 2 M. Rich,
Introduction Relative weights can be estimated by fitting a linear model using responses from individual trials: where g is the linking function. Relative.
Successful Harvest Reporting and Human Dimensions of Waterfowl Hunting Regulations: How to make things work Craig A. Miller Human Dimensions Research Program.
Results Several scores were calculated for each method. The detection rate of latent stains (Figure 1) was calculated based on the visibility of latent.
Introduction to 3D Graphics Lecture 1: Illusions and the Fine Art of Approximation Anthony Steed University College London.
The Effects of Plumage Coloration and Cooing on Mate Attraction in Diamond Doves (Geopelia cuneata) Victoria Hope Department of Biology, York College of.
Gender Influences on Gait when Walking Normally versus Carrying Weight Andrea L. Homan, 1 Chelsea L. Smith, 2 Emily E. Heffernen, 2 Elena E. Leon, 3 David.
1 Extreme Ultraviolet Polarimetry Utilizing Laser-Generated High- Order Harmonics N. Brimhall, M. Turner, N. Herrick, D. Allred, R. S. Turley, M. Ware,
Perception Chapter 3 Light is necessary but not sufficient for vision Ganzfeld: a visual field completely lacking in contour, or luminance changes. Prolonged.
Conclusions There was no significant difference in deer activity at night and day or between lunar cycles. Deer may have a weak tendency to be more active.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS REFERENCES -Are view-boundaries special – eliciting extrapolation? There are other boundaries within a view that have similar characteristics,
Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences (5 th ed.) Gravetter & Wallnau Chapter 17 The Chi-Square Statistic: Tests for Goodness of Fit and Independence University.
Neighborhood Operations
Visual Electrodiagnostic Testing-8 Mutez Gharaibeh,MD.
3.2 VISION 70% of your receptor cells are in your eyes taste and touch need direct contact where as sight and smell don’t Sight can be experienced from.
Acknowledgements: This research was supported by the USDA-ARS Specific Cooperative Agreement Biologically Based Weed Management for Organic Farming Systems.
Mondrians, Aesthetics, and the Horizontal Effect 1 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville 2 Department of Ophthalmology.
 Introduction  Surface Albedo  Albedo on different surfaces  Seasonal change in albedo  Aerosol radiative forcing  Spectrometer (measure the surface.
Contrast and Low Vision Moorfields Eye Hospital, UCL & Kingston University London Rubin, Dalke, Corso and Riaz Research funded by Special Trustees of Moorfields.
Self-assessment Accuracy: the influence of gender and year in medical school self assessment Elhadi H. Aburawi, Sami Shaban, Margaret El Zubeir, Khalifa.
Goal and Motivation To study our (in)ability to detect inconsistencies in the illumination of objects in images Invited Talk! – Hany Farid: Photo Forensincs:
An Introduction to Analyzing Colors in a Digital Photograph Rob Snyder.
Development of Vegetation Indices as Economic Thresholds for Control of Defoliating Insects of Soybean James BoardVijay MakaRandy PriceDina KnightMatthew.
Results I) Regional Survey Rarefaction curves leveled off across sites, suggesting that the sample effort was sufficient to capture differences between.
Does the Male’s Seductive Song Overpower His Appearance? Sexual Selection in Zebra Finches, Taeniopygia guttata Erin Moore Department of Biology, York.
Male Mate Preference Female rump and chest coloration does not influence male mate preference: Figure 4: Average male preference for each treatment (1)
Male mate choice based on sexually selected traits in female eastern bluebirds, Sialia sialis Joanna K. Hubbard & John P. Swaddle Biology Department, College.
Early Identification of Introductory Major's Biology Students for Inclusion in an Academic Support Program BETHANY V. BOWLING and E. DAVID THOMPSON Department.
The Impact of Nutrients on Picophytoplankton Populations Along the Atlantic Coast Melinda Norris and Dr. Jessica Nolan Conclusions  The phytoplankton.
Female coloration, sexual selection, and male mate choice in eastern bluebirds, Sialia sialis Joanna K. Hubbard & John P. Swaddle Biology Department, College.
Chapter 9: Perceiving Color. Figure 9-1 p200 Figure 9-2 p201.
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 32 Biological Heading, Color.
Effects of Stream Restoration: A Comparative Study of Pine Run in Felton, Pennsylvania Luke Mummert, Department of Biological Sciences, York College of.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(13):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure 1. Left – a small region of a typical polarized spectrum acquired with the ESPaDOnS instrument during the MiMeS project. This figure illustrates.
Comparisons of Hunting Seasons and Light Goose Conservation Order Participation and Harvest in the Central and Mississippi Flyways, H.M. Johnson,
Color Image Processing
Color Image Processing
David Marchant, Evelyn Carnegie, Paul Ellison
Assessing Students' Understanding of the Scientific Process Amy Marion, Department of Biology, New Mexico State University Abstract The primary goal of.
Color Image Processing
Intro To Waterfowl Education
Color Representation Although we can differentiate a hundred different grey-levels, we can easily differentiate thousands of colors.
Computer Vision Lecture 4: Color
Within a Mixed-Frequency Visual Environment
Color Image Processing
Retinal Representation of the Elementary Visual Signal
Volume 51, Issue 1, Pages (July 2006)
Team Maggot: Designing a Better Bait Cup
The ribozyme approach distinguishes RNA-dependent and RNA-independent functions of lincRNA genes. The ribozyme approach distinguishes RNA-dependent and.
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Examining Environmental Injustice in Florida
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Volume 89, Issue 2, Pages (August 2005)
Timescales of Inference in Visual Adaptation
Daniel E. Winkowski, Eric I. Knudsen  Neuron 
The Normalization Model of Attention
by Kenneth W. Latimer, Jacob L. Yates, Miriam L. R
Chapter 14: Light Section 2: Light and Color
Setback area relative to drainage area Runoff volume, mean of 4 events
Selective and coherent activity increases due to stimulation indicate functional distinctions between episodic memory networks by Sungshin Kim, Aneesha.
Northern shovelers may have unique microbiome differences with respect to IAV infection, relative to the other duck species. Northern shovelers may have.
Museum Lighting: Adjusting The Illuminant
Presentation transcript:

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Harvest Data The total number of ducks harvested were 105 and 78, and the total number of shots taken were 403 and 265 using UV decoys and non-UV decoys, respectively. However, there were no significant differences in number of ducks harvested per hunter hour (p=0.34) nor shots taken per hunter hour (p=0.49) given the combined data across all three years (Fig. 4). While other decoy technologies substantially impact wildlife harvest rates (Ackerman et al 2006), our data do not show significant increases in harvest rates of waterfowl. Color Comparisons Reflectance data of decoy colors shows obvious differences between UV-paint and non-UV paint, with the former reflecting more ultraviolet light, and obvious differences between both paints and real duck plumage (Fig. 2). Visual modeling of color discrimination indicates that both UV and non-UV decoy colors are equally different to a waterfowl visual system compared to real duck plumage colors (Table 1). Given that analyses of color discrimination from the perspective of waterfowl visual capabilities indicate that UV and non-UV decoy colors are essentially visually identical to ducks, this provides a potential reasonable explanation for the lack of significant differences in hunter harvest data. The Effectiveness of Innovative Wildlife Harvest Tools II: Decoy Colors and Waterfowl Hunter Success INTRODUCTION For centuries, decoys have been used to harvest wildlife, and recent technological advances have resulted in more effective waterfowl hunting decoys, such as spinning-wing decoys (Ackerman et al. 2006) Unrelated research has demonstrated that all birds see ultraviolet wavelengths (Fig. 1; Cuthill et al. 2000), which humans do not see. Recognition of visual differences between birds and humans has led to development of waterfowl decoy paint colors that aim to resemble duck feather colors as birds would see them, through the inclusion of UV-reflectance (see Fig. 2). Our study: 1. From an avian visual perspective, do these new UV- paint colors more closely resemble real duck plumage colors compared to traditional, non-UV paints? 2. Does use of decoys with UV-paint colors result in higher harvest rates of waterfowl? Scott K. Anliker, Luke C. Campillo, and Muir D. Eaton Drake University, College of Arts and Sciences, Department of Biology Figure 1. Spectral sensitivity functions for the different single cone cells found in the retina of waterfowl (top) and humans (bottom). Note the VS cone of birds is sensitive to UV wavelengths ( nm), while human vision is restricted to wavelengths above 400nm (adapted from Cuthill et al. 2000). METHODS Color Comparisons Reflectance data of drake Mallards (Anas platyrhynchos) was collected from the head, neck ring, speculum border, speculum, tertial, flank, rump, and outer tail regions on 24 UV decoys, 24 non- UV decoys, and 20 real ducks from museum specimens (Fig. 2). We calculated the linear distance between two colors in avian perceptual color space (ΔS) using the Vorobyev-Osorio (1998) color discrimination model. Model parameters include spectral sensitivity functions for single cone cells (Fig. 1), noise-to-signal ratios of single cone cells given their relative abundances, and reflectance data of colors to be compared. For detailed calculations, see Wilson et al By definition threshold for color discrimination is ΔS=1jnd (just noticeable difference), where two colors are barely distinguishable under ideal viewing conditions. ΔS from 1 to 3 represent color differences that are difficult to distinguish, and ΔS>3 represent easily distinguishable colors (Table 1). Harvest Data Volunteer hunters were solicited to participate in data collection to assess the effectiveness of UV decoys. All hunting day trials took place at the Chichaqua Bottoms Greenbelt Controlled Waterfowl Hunting area, Polk Co., IA (Fig. 3). This area consists of 13 permanent hunting blinds in fixed locations on an approximately 120- acre hunting marsh. On a given hunting day, volunteer hunting groups were given either four dozen UV decoys or four dozen non-UV decoys, and these were used for hunting from sunrise to 1pm at one of the 13 permanent hunting blinds. Hunters recorded shots taken, hours hunted, and ducks harvested. All duck species were included in total ducks harvested. We calculated shots per duck hunter hour and ducks harvested per duck hunter hour for each hunting trial. Data was pooled from 106 total hunting trials across the 2008, 2009, and 2010 Iowa waterfowl hunting seasons for decoy treatment comparisons (Fig. 4). Using a student’s t-test, we tested for differences between UV and non-UV decoy ducks harvested, ducks harvested per hunter hour, shots taken, and shots taken per hunter hour. Figure 2. Example decoys used in field trials (A), and reflectance data from the white tail (B) and grey body (C) colors. Note that the two decoys are visually identical to humans (A), although, one is painted with UV-colors while the other is painted with traditional non-UV colors. Comparing reflectance data of the UV-paints, non-UV paints, and real duck plumage reveals the differences in UV reflectance for both the white tail colors (B) and the grey body colors (C). Given Figure 1, these differences in UV reflectance should result in different colors seen by the ducks (see Table 1). Diamonds represent non-UV decoys, X’s UV decoys, and triangles real mallard plumage. LITERATURE CITED: 1. Ackerman, J.T. et al J. of Wildlife Management. 70: Cuthill, I.C. et al Advan. Study Behav. 29: Vorobyev, M. and D. Osorio Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265: Wilson, R.E. et al Orn. Neotrop. 19: ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Doug Sheeley for permission to collect data on the controlled hunting marsh at Chichaqua Bottoms Greenbelt, and for use of facilities for storage of equipment; all volunteer hunters that participated in data collection; Flambeau Outdoors, Inc. for an equipment grant to support this research, and Drake University for financial support. Figure 3. Photos from Chichaqua Bottoms Greenbelt study site. Top shows typical placement of decoys for a given trial day. Bottom shows typical permanent blind used for hunting trials. Figure 4. Hunter success, as measured through average ducks harvested per hunter hour and shots taken per hunter hour. Data compiled for 2008, 2009, and 2010 hunting seasons, with error bars representing 95% CI. Black bars represent UV decoy use; white bars represent non-UV decoy use. A Wavelength (nm) % Reflectance C B # / hunter hour Wavelength (nm) UV vs. RealNon-UV vs. RealUV vs. Non-UV Head Neck ring Speculum Edge Speculum Tertials Flanks Rump Outer tail Table 1. Color discrimination (ΔS) values using the Vorobyev-Osorio (1998) model comparing colors from UV decoys, non-UV decoys, and real duck plumage. From an avian visual perspective, ΔS=1 represents threshold for color discrimination, ΔS from 1 to 3 represent difficult to distinguish color differences, and ΔS>3 represent easily distinguishable colors Relative Sensitivity Waterfowl visual modeling also indicates that UV and non-UV decoy colors are either not distinguishable as different, or are very difficult to distinguish as different, from each other (Table 1).