That’s too bad… NOT! Predictors of feeling pleasure in response to another’s misfortune Danielle Ryan, Katey Quigley, Stacy Miller, Amanda Werner, Amy.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Study 1: Beliefs About Age Variation in Mating Strategies and Partner Preferences Studies 2 and 3: Actual Age Variation in Mating Strategies and Partner.
Advertisements

Research Method Interviews -Asked for basic demographic information * race, ethnicity, year, socioeconomic status - Views on race relations on campus.
Brewer, B. W., Selby, C.L., Linder, D.E., & Petitpas, A.J. (1999)
An Analysis of Personality Type and Relationship Desirability Within Hook-up Culture: Nice-Guys vs. Bad-Boys An Analysis of Personality Type and Relationship.
The Philosophy of Exotischism An Overview 85 In the period following World War II there were many books and articles published that showed how.
AGE VARIATION IN MATING STRATEGIES AND MATE PREFERENCES AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS Danielle Ryan and April Bleske-Rechek, University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire.
Relationships Psychology.

HAPPINESS QUOTIENT What's your happiness quotient???
Friendship and Support. Overview of Friendship Nature of Friendship Rules of Friendship Theories of Friendship Balance Theory Developmental Theory Theories.
The Double-Edged Nature of Friendship: Attractiveness and Rivalry in Women’s Friendships Katherine Quigley and Carolyn Kolb, University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Can You Match These Friends? A Test of Genetic Similarity Theory Katrina M. Sandager, Stephanie R. A. Maves, Sarah L. Hubert, and April Bleske-Rechek University.
Schadenfreude: Contexts in Which Men and Women Feel Pleasure in Response to Others' Misfortune Bethany Franklin, Carly Murray, and Arianna Brown Faculty.
Introduction Who’s to blame? Effects of Perpetrator Gender and Victim Confrontation on Perceptions of Blame Variables Results Method Domestic violence:
Abstract Deciding the topic of SES Kristen Morgan & Caitlin Vandre-Schmidt  Psychology  University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire Kristen Morgan & Caitlin Vandre-Schmidt.
Conflict, Supportive Communication, and Group Satisfaction Katlynn Balson, Laura Turner, Virgil Ward II, Alexandra Zaic Faculty Mentor: Dr. Martha Fay.
Unfaithful: Examining Infidelity in Adolescent Romantic Relationships Rebecca E. Furr, Hannah G. Arick, & Deborah P. Welsh University of Tennessee.
Method IntroductionResults Discussion Effects of Plans and Workloads on Academic Performance Mark C. Schroeder University of Nebraska – Lincoln College.
Genetic Factors Predisposing to Homosexuality May Increase Mating Success in Heterosexuals Written by Zietsch et. al By Michael Berman and Lindsay Tooley.
Evolutionary Psychology, Workshop 11: Controllability of Mate Value.

Boosting our self esteem: A need to gossip Kristine Barnes-Meyers Dawn Mohar.
Project Background Results Amanda Krueger & Tiffany Huynh  Dr. Robert J. Eierman  ORSP  University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire prioritieschallenges The.
Attraction and Flirtation in Young Adults’ and Middle-Aged Adults’ Opposite-Sex Friendships Erin E. Hirsch, Cierra A. Micke, and April Bleske-Rechek University.
D OES R EARING E NVIRONMENT S HAPE R OMANTIC A TTACHMENT S TYLE ? U SING S IBLINGS TO T EST FOR S HARED E NVIRONMENTAL I NFLUENCES Amy E. Johnson, Bethany.
The role of self-handicapping in social networks Bridgett J. Milner Edward R. Hirt Thanks to: Kristin Hendrix & Erin Steury.
Exploring Honors Students’ Levels of Academic Motivation, Perfectionism, and Test Anxiety Hannah Geis, Kelly Hughes, and Brittany Weber, Faculty Advisor:
Romantic Red: Red Enhances Men’s Attraction to Women Andrew J. Elliot and Daniela Niesta University of Rochester Elliot, A., & Niesta, D. (2008 November).
Exploring the Relationships Among College Students’ Goal Orientations, Perfectionism, and Academic Self-Efficacy Hannah Geis and Brittany Weber, Faculty.
POSTER TEMPLATE BY: om Sex Differences in Associations between Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE) and Substance Use Lesley A.
Origins of Attraction MATTHEW CORRINET. Biological: Fischer et al. (2003)  “... used an fMRI... to investigate blood flow in the brains of 20 men and.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © The Homework Effect: Does Homework Help or Harm Students? Katherine Field EdD Candidate, Department.
A TTRACTION AND A TTRACTIVENESS IN M ALE -F EMALE D YADS : D O D ATING C OUPLES D IFFER FROM C ROSS -S EX F RIENDS ? Whitney Joseph, Heather Williquette,
Figure 1. Display of the percentage of men and women in each age group who spontaneously nominated mating desires as a benefit of their friendships (left)
Methods of Media Research Communication covers a broad range of topics. Also it draws heavily from other fields like sociology, psychology, anthropology,
Individual Preferences for Uncertainty: An Ironically Pleasurable Stimulus Bankert, M., VanNess, K., Hord, E., Pena, S., Keith, V., Urecki, C., & Buchholz,
Friends as Rivals: Perceptions of Attractiveness Predict Mating Rivalry in Female Friendships Stephanie R. A. Maves, Sarah L. Hubert, and April Bleske-Rechek.
Introduction Wellness Programs and the Effects on Job Satisfaction Alexiss Jeffers, Kendra Jirschele, Molly O’Hearn, April Palmer, Alyssa Wichman, Dr.
Friends (Temporarily) Forever: Frequency of Facebook Use, Relationship Satisfaction, and Perception of Friendship Zack Hayes, Jerad Hill, Heather Jacobson,
By: Deanna Duermit, Mikayla Mowzoon, Jenna Tioseco
This research was supported by funding from the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs at the University of Wisconsin- Eau Claire. References Study.
On Your Own, Jot Down… 1. Describe the earliest memory you have of an experience with a person or people of a cultural or ethnic group different from your.
Attractive Equals Smart? Perceived Intelligence as a Function of Attractiveness and Gender Abstract Method Procedure Discussion Participants were 38 men.
Can Peer Pressure Be A Good Thing? Megan M. Schad, Meredyth A. Evans, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
By Chris Anscombe Evaluation Question 3.
The Broader Context of Relational Aggression in Adolescent Romantic Relationships Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Amanda Hare, Jill Antonishak, Joseph.
Figure 1. Percent of Couples Still Dating at Time 2, as a Function of Their Relationship Commitment at Time 1. Trolley Problem Decisions Follow the Laws.
Introduction Disordered eating continues to be a significant health concern for college women. Recent research shows it is on the rise among men. Media.
Sex differences in romantic kissing among college students: An evolutionary perspective Summary by Amber Kika, Nina Dangourian, and Esmeralda Huerta For.
Sex Differences in Jealousy: The Effect of Imagination Sarah Strout, James Laird, Ph. D., Nicholas Thompson, Ph. D., Sarah Bush & Aaron Shafer Study 1.
Parents' Marital Functioning and the Development of Adolescent Romantic Relationships Amanda L. Hare, F. Christy McFarland, & Joseph P. Allen University.
Gender & IT Education Gender and IT Education Conference, Indiana University, 2007 Gender & IT Education The Importance of Mentors Faculty and Student.
MODEL 2 MODEL 1 Secular, but not Religious, Coping Predicts Self-Control Gretchen Schultz & Tara Poncelet Faculty Collaborator: Jeffrey Goodman, Ph.D.
Results Time 2 (Age 18-20) Target teen and their romantic partner engaged in an 8 minute hypothetical disagreement task interaction. Hostile, relationship-undermining.
Introduction Method United States Belgium Discussion Katherine Quigley & Emily Prosser Faculty Mentors: Dr. Jennifer Muehlenkamp, Department of Psychology,
Studies that Illustrate Errors in Attributions IB Psychology I (1A)
We thank the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for supporting this research, and Learning & Technology Services for printing this poster. Introduction.
Overview of Results and Demographics Amanda Krueger  Dr. Robert J. Eierman  ORSP  University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire prioritieschallenges The Mindful.
Female college students reporting satisfaction with the shape of their bodies were unlikely to choose their own identified silhouette as their ideal body.
ABSTRACT A fictitious company asked participants to fill out a marketing survey in exchange for a gift card to a local movie theater; cards either expired.
Buffers to Online Harassment in Women’s Pursuit of STEM Careers Rachael Bryant, Jennifer Hightower and, Bryan Dawson Ph.D Background: Women have consistently.
W HEN M ISFORTUNE B EFALLS A NOTHER Cassandra L. Abel, Amanda L. Johnson, and April Bleske-Rechek University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire B ACKGROUND Schadenfreude.
Better to Give or to Receive?: The Role of Dispositional Gratitude
Forgiving In the Moment
Attraction and Attractiveness in a Naturally Occurring
Friendship Quality as a Moderator
Natural Sampling versus Mental Concepts Whitney Joseph
Effects of Sexualization in Advertisements
Presentation transcript:

That’s too bad… NOT! Predictors of feeling pleasure in response to another’s misfortune Danielle Ryan, Katey Quigley, Stacy Miller, Amanda Werner, Amy Johnson, Carolyn Kolb UWEC Department of Psychology, Faculty mentor: Dr. April Bleske-Rechek MISFORTUNE DOMAIN MenWomen ME Sex F ME Target F Sex x Target FOverall Pleasure Non-friend in Network Close FriendNon-friend in Network Close Friend Academic (e.g., He/she is usually at the top of the class but got beat out by somebody else.) 1.71 (2.28)0.88 (1.45)1.96 (2.44)1.08 (1.76) ** (2.03) Appearance (e.g, He/she gained a noticeable amount of weight.) 1.19 (1.73)1.08 (1.59)2.06 (2.53)0.96 (1.51) * (1.97) Social (e.g., He/she wasn’t invited to some of her friends’ social gatherings.) 0.84 (1.63)0.74 (1.45)1.86 (2.45)0.78 (1.59) * (1.93) Physical (e.g., He/she was normally the best athlete on the team but had a string of bad performances.) 1.08 (1.52)0.84 (1.37)1.67 (2.18)0.80 (1.44) * (1.74) Mating (e.g., He/she began to have a hard time hooking up with women/men.) 1.46 (2.05)1.59 (2.18)1.54 (2.35)0.54 (1.40) *1.07 (1.94) Material (e.g., He/she could no longer afford to buy clothes from the stores he/she wanted to.) 1.14 (1.58)0.86 (1.38)1.32 (1.90)0.57 (1.22) * (1.56) Career (e.g., He/she got reprimanded at work for slacking off.) 1.31 (2.07)0.53 (0.95)1.39 (1.90)0.59 (1.37) ** (1.62) Romantic Partner (e.g., His/her romantic partner had sex with someone else.) 1.62 (2.53)0.82 (1.32)1.24 (1.83)0.49 (1.07) *** (1.59) 1. Dislike – and not envy – predicts pleasure felt at another’s misfortune. They also envied their friend more than they envied the non-friend in their social network. In fact, the more people liked the target, the more they envied the target, r(305) =.32, p <.001. However, dislike – and not envy – predicts schadenfreude. Envy toward a person did not predict feeling pleasure in response to their misfortunes, but disliking them did predict feeling pleasure in response to that person experiencing misfortunes. Both men and women liked their friend a lot more than the non- friend in their social network. BACKGROUND “Schadenfreude” is a feeling of pleasure in response to another’s misfortune (Feather, 1989). Men and women are more likely to feel schadenfreude toward members of the same sex, presumably because they are comparing themselves to members of the same sex (van Dijk et al., 2006). Some researchers have suggested that people feel schadenfreude toward people they envy (who have what they would like to have) (Smith, 1991). It is possible that both dislike (or feelings of hostility) and envy toward a hypothetical target can elicit happiness in response to imagining them experiencing a misfortune (van Dijk et al., 2006). However, in research using focused scenarios to manipulate envy and dislike, researchers have shown that dislike for a hypothetical target elicits more schadenfreude than does envy (Hareli & Weiner, 2002). Previous research has used focused scenarios involving a single misfortune, usually in the academic domain (e.g., a person at the top of the class failing the final exam). The current research had three objectives: To determine, via act nomination, a range of domains in which men and women are likely to experience schadenfreude (Phase I); To examine differences in men’s and women’s reported pleasure in response to imagining a same-sex target experience a range of misfortunate events (Phase II); To examine men’s and women’s differential reaction to imagining misfortunes befalling a close same-sex friend as opposed to a same-sex member of their social network (Phase II). RESULTS 2. Men and women report low levels of schadenfreude overall, but different misfortune domains elicit different amounts of pleasure. METHOD Phase I An initial sample of 40 students from two psychology courses were polled about either (a) circumstances in which they had felt envy toward another or (b) circumstances in which they had felt a touch of pleasure in response to someone else’s misfortune. Students were asked to describe three specific circumstances in which they had felt the emotion. The circumstances that students nominated spanned a variety of domains and served as an initial list from which we expanded. Friends of the same sex and family members were the main targets in the circumstances that students mentioned; strangers rarely appeared. We split the nominations into domains and, by informally surveying friends of the research team, we generated several specific examples for each domain. Phase II In Phase II, 68 men and 241 women (mean age = 21.59) completed an online questionnaire about “mixed emotions.” We established events on various Facebook group pages and sent the survey link to students who participate in research for course credit. Participants were asked to think of either (a) a close same sex friend or (b) a member of the same sex who was a member of their social network but whom they would not consider to be a friend (henceforth “non-friend”). They typed this person’s name into a response box, and that name was inserted into subsequent questions pertaining to that person. Participants imagined each of 44 different events happening to their target person (same-sex friend or same-sex non-friend). In response to each item, participants reported how much sympathy they would feel, and then how much pleasure they would feel. All ratings were on an 11-point scale (0=None to 10=A lot). Sympathy ratings were included to facilitate admission of pleasure in response to the events. Table 1 shows a sample event from each domain, and the overall mean pleasure rating for that domain. Before concluding the survey, participants used Likert type rating scales to report the degree to which they liked or disliked the target person, how much they envied the person, how much they felt in competition with the person, and how emotionally close they were to the person. 3. Overall, women tend to discriminate between friends and non-friends more than men do. A common pattern in the data (particularly in the domains shown in the three figures above) was for men to report a low level of pleasure in response to misfortunes experienced by either a friend or a non-friend in their network. Women on the other hand, showed a peak in pleasure in response to a non-friend experiencing various misfortune. In other words, men were less affected by whom they were thinking about than women were. Not shown here is the corresponding pattern for sympathy. Men reported similarly moderate levels of sympathy in response to misfortunes experienced by either target, and women showed a peak level of sympathy in response to a friend experiencing misfortunes, with little sympathy for non-friends. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Dr. April Bleske-Rechek’s students for sharing circumstances in which they experience envy and schadenfreude. We thank the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for supporting this research and Learning & Technology Services for the printing of this poster. REFERENCES Benenson, J. F., Hodgson, L., Heath, S., & Welch, P. J. (2008) Human sexual differences in the use of social ostracism as competitive tactic. International Journal of Primatology, 29, Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Feather, N. T. (1989). Attitudes towards the high achiever: The fall of the tall poppy. Australian Journal of Psychology, 41, Feather, N. T. & Nairn, K. (2005). Resentment, envy, schadenfreude and sympathy: Effects of own and other’s deserved or undeserved status. Australian Journal of Psychology, 57, Hareli, S. & Weiner, B. (2002). Dislike and envy as antecedents and another’s misfortune. Motivation and Emotion, 26, van Dijk, W. W., Ouwerkerk, J. W. Goslinga, S., Nieweg, M., & Gallucci, M. (2006). When people fall from grace: Reconsidering the role of envy in schadenfreude. Emotion, 6, Smith, R. H. (1991). Envy and the sense of injustice. In P. Salovey (Ed.), Psychology of jealousy and envy (pp ). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. DISCUSSION The current series of studies extended existing research on schadenfreude in two primary ways. First, we asked participants to respond about misfortunes across a variety of domains instead of focusing on just one (most previous research focuses on academic failure as a misfortune). Second, we asked men and women to think about a specific person they know rather an imaginary target. We found that although participants envied their friends more than their non-friends, they didn’t experience more schadenfreude toward their friends than their non-friends. In fact, dislike – and not envy -- predicted schadenfreude. This finding supports recent arguments that the roots of schadenfreude are not in envy, as commonly thought, but rather in feelings of dislike and hostility (e.g., Hareli & Weiner, 2002). Previous research has not documented sex differences in reported pleasure over others’ misfortunes. However, we documented circumstances in which women reported more schadenfreude toward a non-friend member of their network, and those circumstances were tied to dimensions on which women are known to compete. First, women compete on attractiveness (Buss, 1988), and their peak schadenfreude was toward non-friends experiencing a decrease in their attractiveness or physical fitness. Second, females from an early age compete to occupy limited slots in small groups (Benenson et al., 2008), and women reported relatively high schadenfreude toward non-friends experiencing social ostracism. Our finding that men differentiated less than women did between targets in their emotional responses to misfortune needs to be replicated. It is entirely possible that men actually do not differentiate between different types of relationships, but we had far fewer men than women in the sample so our power to detect differences in men’s responses was limited. One surprising element of the study was the low level of schadenfreude that people reported despite the fact that we told participants that it is common for people to experience both sympathy and pleasure toward another’s misfortune. While it is possible that our participants were displaying a classic level of Midwestern “niceness,” we think it is more likely that participants truly experience less schadenfreude when imagining a real person – as they did in our study – rather than an imaginary target. Note. Internal reliabilities by domain exceeded.80.