„Ostention effect” in language acquisition experiments Katalin É. Kiss, Mátyás Gerőcs, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Language Processing Hierarchy
Advertisements

Faculty of Arts University of Groningen The acquisition of the weak-strong distinction and the Dutch quantifier allemaal Erik-Jan Smits
Hungarian preschoolers’ interpretation of doubly quantified sentences Katalin É. Kiss, Mátyás Gerőcs, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics.
Quantifier spreading: children misled by ostensive cues
Theories of Classical Conditioning
Language Acqisition - From Womb to School. Content Pre/Postnatal Language Development The First Three Years The Pre-School Years The School Years.
The truth shall make you slow: Superlative Quantifiers as speech act modifiers Aviya Hacohen, Dana Kozlowski & Ariel Cohen Ben-Gurion University of the.
Chapter 5 Semantics The First Week.
Online processing of bidirectional optimization Petra Hendriks, Jacolien van Rij & Hedderik van Rijn Tandem Workshop on Optimality in Language and Geometric.
Bruner’s Approach Objectives: Outline Bruner’s concept of scaffolding.
Focus affected quantification in adult and child langage Erik-Jan Smits Semantics in the Netherlands Day Utrecht University of Groningen, Dutch.
Topics in Cognition and Language: Theory, Data and Models *Perceptual scene analysis: extraction of meaning events, causality, intentionality, Theory of.
Linguistics / Communication Disorders Thomas Roeper Barbara Zurer Pearson Margaret Grace University of Massachusetts Amherst
Module 5 Writing the Results and Discussion (Chapter 3 and 4)
Domain restriction in child language Erik-Jan Smits 1, Tom Roeper 2 and Bart Hollebrandse 1 1 University of Groningen, The Netherlands 2 University of.
1 Human simulations of vocabulary learning Présentation Interface Syntaxe-Psycholinguistique Y-Lan BOUREAU Gillette, Gleitman, Gleitman, Lederer.
Discrimination-Shift Problems Background This type of task has been used to compare concept learning across species as well as across a broad range of.
Chapter 2: Piaget's Stages of Cognitive Development Jean Piaget ( )
Chapter Two Miss.Mona AL-Kahtani. Why do people study language acquisition??? Take a minute and think about it?
Chapter 4 Learning: Theories and Program Design
Cognitive Development of Preschoolers
Feldman Child Development, 3/e ©2004 Prentice Hall Chapter 9 Cognitive Development in the Preschool Years Child Development, 3/e by Robert Feldman Created.
Cognitive Development Cognitive development refers to the growth and change of a person’s ability to process information, solve problems and gain knowledge.
RESEARCH DESIGN.
Aligning developmental and educational diagnosis: Principles and implications Andreas Demetriou University of Nicosia, Cyprus Presented at the conference.
An investigation of Conservativity Tim Hunter Anastasia Conroy.
Piaget’s lifePiaget’s life Born SwitzerlandPhDBinet.
McGraw-Hill © 2006 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved. The Nature of Research Chapter One.
Cognitive Development: Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s Theories
General Knowledge Dr. Claudia J. Stanny EXP 4507 Memory & Cognition Spring 2009.
Piagetian Theory of Cognition (Pointers From Reviews) By Grace Nwosu Assistant Professor, Curriculum and Instruction.
LEARNING GOAL 9.2: PREDICT THE RATIONAL ABILITIES AND LIMITATIONS OF A CHILD BASED ON PIAGET'S COGNITIVE STAGES. Cognitive Development.
Speech and Language Issues For Babies and Pre-school age children who have Down Syndrome Ups and Downs Southwest Conference 2007.
Chapter 1: Research in the Behavioral Sciences History of Behavioral Research Aristotle and Buddha questioned human nature and why people behave in certain.
Big Idea 1: The Practice of Science Description A: Scientific inquiry is a multifaceted activity; the processes of science include the formulation of scientifically.
Dr. Ramez. Bedwani.  Different methods of learning  Factors affecting learning.
The human 3 of 3 1 Lecture 4 chapter 1 the human 3 of 3.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Learning Words and Rules Abstract Knowledge of Word Order in Early Sentence Comprehension Yael Gertner.
Techniques of research control: -Extraneous variables (confounding) are: The variables which could have an unwanted effect on the dependent variable under.
Chapter 8 Language & Thinking
Preschool Cognitive Development.  3 Year Old  Short sentences  896 Words  Great growth in communication  Tells simple stories  Uses words as tool.
Cognitive Information Processing Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos.
Psycholinguistic Theory
The human 3 of 3 U2Mvo&feature=player_embedded the human 3 of 31.
LEARNING DISABILITIES IMPACTING MATHEMATICS Ann Morrison, Ph.D.
C ONTEXT AND CULTURE. D O YOU REMEMBER THIS ? Hymes suggests that in order to be able to communicate language, a person should acquire four types of knowledge:
LEXICAL LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION IN CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME Abstract LEXICAL LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION IN CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME Elbouz M.
Ostention effect in experiments testing children’s interpretation of quantification Katalin É. Kiss, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for.
A methodological problem of language acquisition studies Katalin É. Kiss, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of the Hungarian.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
When is children’s scope interpretation non-isomorphic, and why? Katalin É. Kiss & Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of.
Linguistic Development Thomas G. Bowers, Ph.D
Model of Memory RETRIEVAL Turning now to Long-Term Memory ATTENTION
Early Years Foundation Stage
What is Science? Too broad? Think about the classes devoted to “science” that we study in school. Science is defined as knowledge about or study of the.
Piaget’s Theory of Conservation
Unit 11: Use observation, assessment and planning
What does the speaker mean when s/he utters a sentence? Berg (1993): “What we understand from an utterance could never be just the literal meaning of the.
Creative Curriculum and GOLD Assessment: Early Childhood Competency Based Evaluation System By Carol Bottom.
© 2013 by McGraw-Hill Education. This is proprietary material solely for authorized instructor use. Not authorized for sale or distribution in any manner.
SCIENTIFIC INQUIRY CHAPTER 1 SECTION 2 PHYSICAL SCIENCE.
Chapter 2 sect 1 Objectives List and describe the steps of the experimental method. Describe why a good hypothesis is not simply a guess. Describe the.
Chapter 10 Language acquisition Language acquisition----refers to the child’s acquisition of his mother tongue, i.e. how the child comes to understand.
Gaze cues in mother-child dyads Heather Bell and Meredith Meyer University of Oregon INTRODUCTION RESULTS CONCLUSIONS METHODS REFERENCES ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.
Research in Child Development Dr. Amanda Hilsmier.
Competences for science teaching at the 21st century
Selin Gulgoz Susan A. Gelman University of Michigan Introduction
RELEVANCE THEORY Group Members Sana saif Huma Wazir Junaid Ahmed
Scientific Method Lab Mapping.
Presentation transcript:

„Ostention effect” in language acquisition experiments Katalin É. Kiss, Mátyás Gerőcs, Lilla Pintér, Tamás Zétényi Research Institute for Linguistics of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences This research was supported by grant of OTKA, the National Science Research Foundation

Claim: Presenting an utterance and its visual representation to a child in a test situation may elicit reactions that do not occur in natural, everyday circumstances.

The reason for the unexpected reaction: i.the iconicity of the visual representation, ii.or the test situation itself, which may induce the child to interpret the stimuli as ostensive communication.

Two case studies, and two ways of eliminating the ostension effect: (1) Quantifier spreading (2) The interpretation of numerals

Quantifier Spreading 1. Classic spreading Is every girl riding a bicycle? No, not that one.

2. Bunny spreading Is every bunny eating a carrot? No, not that one.

Theories of Quantifier Spreading Cognitive explanation: Immature cognitive (logical) abilities (Inhelder & Piaget 1958, 1964)

Linguistic explanation 1: QS is quantification over (sub)events (Philip 1995) Is every girl riding a bicycle? = ‘Is every subevent an event of a girl riding a bicycle?’ Counter-evidence: QS in non-eventive sentences: Is every girl a bicycle rider? No, not that one.

Linguistic explanation 2: QS is a parsing problem repaired by pragmatics (Drozd 2001, Geurts 2003): Children treat universals as if they were weak quantifiers with no fixed domain. They supply the domain of quantification pragmatically. Replacing (b) with (c): a.Every girl is riding a bicycle. b.[ x: girl(x)] [y: bicycle(y), x rides y] c.[... :... ] [x, y: girl(x), bicycle(y), x rides y]

In fact, QS occurs with weak quantifiers We found that 4 out of 32 children (12,5%) show systematic QS with numerical quantifiers. E.g. Picture showing 3 girls riding bicycles, and an extra bicycle: Are three girls riding a bicycle? No, four.

Linguistic explanation 3: Relevance Account (Philip 2011): Universal quantification triggers exhaustive enumeration verification, which activates symmetrical pattern recognition. The missing object spoiling symmetry is salient for the child, who imagines it to be existent.

A fact not explained by any theory: The frequency of QS can be changed by manipulating the pragmatic conditions: Richer visual or linguistic context (e.g., more than one extra object) or a backgrounded extra object may reduce QS. Both increasing the number of extra objects, and decreasing the size of extra objects?

Hypothesis: Children give non-adult-like answers because they consider all the elements in the visual stimulus relevant. They assume that all the icons represented in the pictures are to be accounted for; their totality constitutes the domain of quantification.

Why? Because they interpret the stimuli as ostensive signals. Children are predisposed to show preferential attention to ostensive communication. They encode the content of ostensive communication as representing relevant episodic information or generalizable knowledge. (Csibra & Gergely 2009, etc.)

Claim: When the visual stimulus in a sentence- picture matching task is a minimal model abstracting away from the details of the situation, children regard the elements of the stimulus as ostensive clues representing all and only the relevant elements to be matched with the sentence.

Experimental evidence Objectives: To show that if the visual stimuli containing only a few icon- like elements are replaced by photos rich in accidental details, they are not misunderstood as ostensive signals, and QS is radically reduced.

Subjects: 46 children from 3 kindergartens Mean age: 5;5 years Adult control: 24 university students, Mean age: 21 years (SD=1,61) Method: Sentence-picture matching; truth value judgement Experimental procedure

Stimuli: 8 test sentences, with a corresponding iconic drawing and a corresponding photo Every child is sitting on a high chair. Experimental procedure

Results: p>0,001

Discussion: Experimenters use iconic stimuli to eliminate irrelevant distractors, to ensure that children are influenced by the relevant, controlled factor(s). This method is mistaken when we want to test whether or not an element in the stimulus is relevant for the linguistic representation. If the visual stimulus is a minimal model devoid of irrelevant details, children tend to interpret all of its elements as ostensive clues to be represented linguistically.

Discussion: If the ostensive effect is diminished by the use of photos taken in natural environments, the proportion of QS is reduced by more than 50%. Why does a richer linguistic or visual context or a backgrounded extra object reduce QS? Both increasing the number of extra objects, and decreasing the size of extra objects reduces the illusion that they are ostensive signals to be represented linguistically.

Conclusion: Quantifier Spreading, studied intensively for the past 50 years, is the artefact of misleading experimental methodology.

Misleading ostension effect in other types of language acquisition experiments Testing scalar implicatures: Mary has 4 cards. entails Mary has 3 cards; Mary has 2 cards; M has 1 card Hence Mary has 3 cards = Mary has at least 3 cards

Exp. 1 testing the interpretation of number phrases Experimenter: Hard-working bears get a reward. Give a candy to the bears that have picked three raspberries.

In this pure test situation proportion of wrong answers: 100% Participants: 20 preschoolers (mean age: 5;6) Results:

Exp. 2 testing number interpretation : Game context; personal involvement of children The experimenter and the child play a card game; the child ends up with 4 identical cards. Experimenter: If you have 3 identical cards, you get a balloon. Child: Sorry, I don’t have three.

In a game-like test situation proportion of wrong answers: 72% Participants: 18 preschoolers (mean age: 5;6) Results:

Exp. 3 testing number interpretation: emphasis not on numbers but on helping Experimenter: Mickey wants to bake an apple pie, but he needs three more apples. Does Donald have three apples?

In this more natural test situation proportion of wrong answers: 35% Participants: 36 preschoolers (mean age: 5;4) Results:

Exp. 4 testing number interpretation: acting out in natural situations Joint activity unrelated to the test task. On the table: 4-5 glasses, cups, sugar lumps in bowl Experimenter: We are thirsty. Can you find 3 glasses on the table? If you can, please, pour us some water.

Exp. 4 testing number interpretation: acting out in natural situations Experimenter: The adults deserve some coffee. Are there 2 cups so that you can pour us some coffee? Experimenter: I drink coffee with 3 lumps of sugar. Are there 3 lumps in the sugar bowl?

In natural acting-out situations proportion of wrong answers: 15% Participants: 46 preschoolers Mean age: 5;5 years Results:

Discussion: In test situations, children interpret numerals as ostensive signals. The ostensive interpretation of numerals blocks their 'at least' reading. The more we hide the test nature of the task, the weaker the blocking effect, the more adult-like the results.

Conclusion: The test situation itself may lead to the ostensive (in this case: literal) interpretation of the stimulus. Acting-out tasks in natural situations are exempt from the ostension effect.

In test situations adults are also susceptible to the ostension effect Research question: Do children/adults understand the exhaustivity of Hungarian focus/English cleft constituent? Truth value judgement of: It was the pie that Grandma baked. Nagyi a tortát sütötte meg.

Baseline: Grandma baked the cake. Nagyi megsütötte a tortát. 53% of adults and 32% of children also rejected the baseline sentence!

Conclusion: In experiments testing children’s sentence interpretation, the stimuli are often misinterpreted as ostensive clues  they may elicit reactions that do not occur in natural circumstances. The cause of misinterpretation: the iconicity of the stimuli the test situation itself.

Ways of eliminating the ostension effect 1.In experiments testing the linguistic relevance of an item, this item must not be the only potentially irrelevant element of the stimulus. 2. Tests should be embedded in natural situations.

References Csibra, G., Gy. Gergely (2009) Natural Pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, Drozd, K.F. (2001) Children’s weak interpretations of universally quantified sentences. In Conceptual Development and Language Acquisition, ed. M. Bowerman and S.C. Levinson, CUP. Gerőcs, M. & Pintér, L. (2014) How do Hungarian preschoolers interpret number words? In: Kohlberger, M., Bellamy, K. & Dutton, E. (eds.): ConSOLE XXI: Proceedings of the 21st Conference of the Student Organization of Linguistics in Europe (8-10 January 2013, Potsdam). Leiden, Leiden University Centre for Linguistics, 104–122. Geurts, B. (2003) Quantifying kids. Language Acquisition 11: Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958) The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Inhelder, B., & Piaget,J. (1964) The Early Growth of Logic in the Child. New York: Harper Philip, W Event quantification in the acquisition of universal quantification. PhD diss., Umass, Amherst. Philip, W Acquiring knowledge of universal quantification. In Handbook of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition, ed. J. de Villiers, T. Roeper, Dordrecht: Springer.