Methods Results Conclusions Selective cholinergic lesions of the medial septum disrupt dead reckoning based navigation M.M. Martin*; M.D. Schultz; S.S.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The bead maze: Adapting the water maze task to the human manipulatory scale E.L. Loda, A.B. Thoennes, J.R. Köppen, S.S. Winter, D.A. Hamilton, D.G. Wallace.
Advertisements

192-IgG Saporin lesions of the medial septum or nucleus basalis magnocellularis disrupt exploratory trip organization. D.G. Wallace*, S.S. Winter, M.M.
AMPHETAMINE AND HALOPERIDOL MAY ALTER TEMPORAL PROCESSING DURING A NATURALLY OCCURRING BEHAVIOR P.S.Wallace 1* ; E.F.Field 2 ; I.Q.Whishaw 2 ; D.G.Wallace.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Blindly Biased: Restricting Cue Access Impacts Spatial Orientation in a Human Analogue of the Water Maze B.P. Apger, D.M.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation disrupts exploratory trip organization D.G. Wallace*; S.K. Knapp; J.A. Silver;
Methods Results Conclusions Walking the plank: Role of the medial septum in distance estimation S.S. Winter; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology,
Humans were able to accurately use dead-reckoning to estimate distance and direction on a smaller scale. Participants modulated their peak speed on the.
A Human Analogue of Rat Food Hoarding Behavior: Searching Path Kinematics Influence the Accuracy of Dead Reckoning S.N. Choudhry; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace*
Conclusions Both groups set up a home base under novel-dark and light conditions and display a similar topographical organization of exploratory trips.
Unilateral lesions of the medial agranular cortex produce a persistent disruption in the organization of food protection behaviors S. J. WAGNER, M. J.
Methods Results Conclusions Mammillothalamic tract lesions disrupt self-movement cue processing in a food hoarding task. *S.S. Winter; J.L. McMillin, D.G.
Methods Results Conclusions Getting lost in the dark: Disruption of directional but not distance estimates following mammillothalamic tract lesions *S.S.
Navigating with fingers and feet: Comparative analysis of human and rat movement kinematics during non-visual spatial tasks J.R. Raines, N.D. McNeal, J.L.
Figure 4: Average dodger speeds are shown for MS (left panel) and NB (right panel) groups. Dodging (circles) and bracing (triangles) speeds are plotted.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Running on rungs: Serial pattern learning on the horizontal ladder *M.J Hylin 1 ; S.S. Winter 1 ; T. L. Marsiglio 1 ;
Figure 5: Initial (left panel) and terminal (right panel) distances between the noses of the dodger and robber rats are plotted separately for dodging.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Does the home base anchor spatial mapping? *D.G. Wallace 1 ; D.A. Hamilton 2 ; A.D. Choate 1 1 Department of Psychology,
Do Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Ever Return Home? S.L.Bennett 1* ; D.A.Hamilton 2 ; D.G.Wallace 1 1. Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb,
Figure 12: Each group’s average correlations are plotted for cued, uncued and dark probes. Group differences were observed during the uncued and dark probes.
Notes: 1. Exam corrections and assignment 3 due today. 2. Last exam – last day of class 3. Chapter 24 reading assignment - pgs. 704 – New website:
Learning like a girl? “Estrogen modulates learning in female rats by acting directly at distinct memory systems” L. Zurkovsky et al. Neuroscience (2007)
Neglecting to protect: DCS lesions disrupt food protection behavior organization. 1 Blankenship, P.A., 1 Wolf, C. N., 2 Cheatwood, J.L., & 1 Wallace, D.G.
Making sense of the senseless: Evaluating sex differences of ASO
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Dmitriy Aronov, David W. Tank  Neuron 
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
A Human Extrastriate Area Functionally Homologous to Macaque V4
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Hiroaki Norimoto, Yuji Ikegaya  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Volume 27, Issue 9, Pages (May 2017)
Jung Hoon Sul, Hoseok Kim, Namjung Huh, Daeyeol Lee, Min Whan Jung 
Volume 75, Issue 4, Pages (August 2012)
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Masahiro Yasuda, Mark R. Mayford  Neuron 
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Attention-Induced Variance and Noise Correlation Reduction in Macaque V1 Is Mediated by NMDA Receptors  Jose L. Herrero, Marc A. Gieselmann, Mehdi Sanayei,
Volume 27, Issue 20, Pages e3 (October 2017)
Hill-Aina Steffenach, Menno Witter, May-Britt Moser, Edvard I. Moser 
Volume 89, Issue 6, Pages (March 2016)
Trajectory Encoding in the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Volume 90, Issue 1, Pages (April 2016)
Volume 38, Issue 5, Pages (June 2003)
Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration
Volume 78, Issue 5, Pages (June 2013)
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Volume 80, Issue 2, Pages (October 2013)
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
James Michael Hyman, Clay Brian Holroyd, Jeremy Keith Seamans  Neuron 
Cortical Motion Deafness
Grid and Nongrid Cells in Medial Entorhinal Cortex Represent Spatial Location and Environmental Features with Complementary Coding Schemes  Geoffrey W.
Direct Two-Dimensional Access to the Spatial Location of Covert Attention in Macaque Prefrontal Cortex  Elaine Astrand, Claire Wardak, Pierre Baraduc,
Wael F Asaad, Gregor Rainer, Earl K Miller  Neuron 
Volume 22, Issue 12, Pages (March 2018)
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (September 2000)
Volume 48, Issue 2, Pages (October 2005)
Traces of Experience in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Volume 22, Issue 12, Pages (March 2018)
Volume 24, Issue 10, Pages (September 2018)
The Hippocampus, Memory, and Place Cells
Acquisition of trace 50–250 is dependent on a subregion of mPFC in mice. Acquisition of trace 50–250 is dependent on a subregion of mPFC in mice. A, CR.
Presentation transcript:

Methods Results Conclusions Selective cholinergic lesions of the medial septum disrupt dead reckoning based navigation M.M. Martin*; M.D. Schultz; S.S. Winter; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb, IL, USA Recent investigations using selective lesion techniques have suggested that the septohippocampal cholinergic system may not be critical for spatial orientation. These studies employ spatial tasks that provide the animal with access to both allothetic and idiothetic cues; therefore, the spared performance may reflect spared spatial orientation or compensatory mechanisms associated with one class of spatial cues. The present study examined the contribution of the septohippocampal cholinergic system to spatial behavior by examining performance in foraging tasks in which cue availability was manipulated. Female Long-Evans rats either received a sham surgery (“control”) or a selective medial septum/ vertical limb of the diagonal band cholinergic lesion using the neurotoxin 192 IgG- saporin (“ms sap”). Rats were then trained to find food pellets randomly located on an open field which they then carried back to a visible refuge (“cued”) to eat. Once they became proficient at returning to their refuge, cued training was alternated with probes. The three probes included 1) replacing the visible refuge with a hidden refuge to measure ability to use cues not associated with the refuge (“uncued”); 2) testing under completely dark conditions thereby limiting access only to idiothetic cues (“dark”); and 3) moving the hidden refuge to a new location to pit use of allothetic cues against idiothetic cues (“reversal”). Trips were divided into two segments for analyses. Outward segments consisted of the moment the rat left the refuge to the moment the rat located the randomly placed food pellet. Homeward segments consisted of the moment the rat retrieved the food pellet until the rat returned to the refuge. Figure 1: Photographs of the testing room are shown with schematic overlays of the table (200 cm diameter) and refuge (17 x 26 x 12cm). Experimental conditions include: Cued (top left), Uncued (top right), Dark (bottom left), and Reversal (bottom right) shown with current refuge (gray box) and former refuge (striped box – Reversal condition only) locations. CuedUncued DarkReversal Figure 3: Topographic characteristics of four trials from a representative control (top panel) and ms sap rat (bottom panel) are shown under cued conditions. Trials were divided into outward (blue) and homeward (red) segments. Locations of the refuge (black box) and food pellets (open circles) are plotted for both rats. Figure 2: Photographs of coronal sections stained for AChE are presented from a representative control (bottom left column) and ms sap (bottom right column) rat. The reduction in AChE was restricted to the hippocampus (right figure). Figure 4: Maximum speed (left panel) and distance ratio (right panel) are plotted for outward and homeward segments under cued conditions. No group differences were observed in either measure. MS Sap Control OutwardHomeward Figure 7: Topographic characteristics of four trials from a representative control (top panel) and ms sap rat (bottom panel) are shown under dark conditions. Figure 5: Topographic characteristics of four trials from a representative control (top panel) and ms sap rat (bottom panel) are shown under uncued conditions. Figure 8: Maximum speed (left panel) and distance ratio (right panel) are plotted for outward and homeward segments under dark conditions. Both groups increased homeward maximum speeds after the first trial. Both groups also took more direct homeward paths after the first trial but overall, the control group took more direct homeward paths than the ms sap group. Figure 6: Maximum speed (left panel) and distance ratio (right panel) are plotted for outward and homeward segments under uncued conditions. No group differences were observed in either measure. OutwardHomeward MS Sap Control OutwardHomeward MS Sap Control Figure 10: Maximum speed (left panel) and distance ratio (right panel) are plotted for outward and homeward segments under reversal conditions. Both groups increased outward maximum speeds on the last trial. Both groups took more direct homeward paths after the first trial but overall, the control group reached higher homeward maximum speeds and took more direct homeward paths than the ms sap group. Figure 9: Topographic characteristics of the first two trials from a representative control (top panel) and ms sap rat (bottom panel) are shown under reversal conditions. Locations of the current refuge (black box), former refuge (striped box), and stops made by the rat after retrieving the food pellet (black triangles) are plotted for both rats. Trip 1 Trip 2 MS Sap Control No group differences were observed when rats were provided access to both allothetic and idiothetic cues under cued and uncued conditions. The MS Sap group was impaired when visual allothetic cues were unavailable under dark conditions. When allothetic and idiothetic cues were put in conflict, the MS Sap group was impaired in returning to the new refuge location under reversal conditions. Results are consistent with MS Sap lesions producing a selective impairment in idiothetic cue processing, and suggests that these rats were able to use allothetic cues to compensate under cued and uncued conditions. Correspondence: M. Martin D. Wallace Web: Support Contributed By: NINDS grant NS Abstract