PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Accessing spoken words: the importance of word onsets
Advertisements

Knowing More than One Language: The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism Marina Blekher Department of Linguistics.
Psycholinguistic what is psycholinguistic? 1-pyscholinguistic is the study of the cognitive process of language acquisition and use. 2-The scope of psycholinguistic.
Psycholinguistics What is psycholinguistics ? Psycholinguistics is the study of the cognitive processes that support the acquisition and use of language.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models cont.
Language Use and Understanding BCS 261 LIN 241 PSY 261 CLASS 12: BRANIGAN ET AL.: PRIMING.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
1 The cognitive psychology of language – 2 Now that we know how words are recognized –How are they produced in the first place? Word production is the.
STM and Encoding Objective – to describe how encoding works in the STM.
Phonological Priming in Spontaneous Speech Production Katrina Housel H uman L anguage P rocessing L ab.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models of language production.
Themes in production Producing speech Reading and writing.
The mental lexicon LG 103 Introduction to psycholinguistics Celia (Vasiliki) Antoniou.
Auditory Word Recognition
Models of word production and reaction-time evidence.
Models of Language Language and Cognition Colombo 2011.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Syntactic Priming in Bilinguals: Effects of verb repetition in an L2-monolingual and cross-lingual setting Sofie Schoonbaert 1, Robert Hartsuiker 1, &
Research presentation Assignment 1 per group: –Prepare a min research presentation of your experiment (power point or overhead, and script of what.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Representing language.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Attention Limited amount of mental resources Mental “resources” = general term could refer mental processes, mental representations, or mental structures.
Mental Lexicon Body of knowledge we hold in our minds about words Includes pronunciation, spelling, meaning syntactic roles Recognition of words—whether.
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 2: Language processing: speed and flexibility.
Sound and Speech. The vocal tract Figures from Graddol et al.
Reading. Reading Research Processes involved in reading –Orthography (the spelling of words) –Phonology (the sound of words) –Word meaning –Syntax –Higher-level.
A Modular Approach to STM Allan Baddeley: Articulatory Loop Central Executive Visuospatial Sketchpad The article by Lee Brooks considers a double-dissociation.
Knowledge information that is gained and retained what someone has acquired and learned organized in some way into our memory.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Theories and models.
Psycholinguistics 05 Internal Lexicon.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Acquisition: Bilinugalism.
Intro to Psycholinguistics What its experiments are teaching us about language processing and production.
Interactions between Language and Stuttering NU/SFA Workshop for Fluency Specialists July, 1996 J. Scott Yaruss, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Pittsburgh.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production & Comprehension: Conversation & Dialog.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Theories & Models.
SPOKEN LANGUAGE COMPREHENSION Anne Cutler Addendum: How to study issues in spoken language comprehension.
Jelena Mirković and Maryellen C. MacDonald Language and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of Wisconsin-Madison Introduction How to Study Subject-Verb.
Retrieval of Lexical-Syntactic Features in Tip-of-the-Tongue States Michele Miozzo and Alfonso Caramazza Presented by Ping Yu.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Last week Characteristics of language production: – Coordinating multiple sources of information in real time Methodological issues.
+ Treatment of Aphasia Week 12 April 1 st, Review Involvement of semantic and phonological stages in naming. Differentiating features of naming.
Age of acquisition and frequency of occurrence: Implications for experience based models of word processing and sentence parsing Marc Brysbaert.
Phonological Encoding II Producingconnectedspeech.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Experimentally elicited speech errors.
The Impact of Exposure to MSA on the Acquisition of Basic Language and Literacy Skills in Arabic Elinor Saiegh-Haddad Bar-Ilan University
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Conversation & Dialog: Language Production and Comprehension in conjoined action.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Stages in lexical access Or: the lemma dilemma. TIGER (X) Tigre Noun Fem. Countable /tigre/ ti g Lexical concept lemma lexeme phonemes Has Stripes Is.
Recent Models of Stuttering Western Illinois University February 7, 1997 J. Scott Yaruss, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Pittsburgh.
Experimental Psychology PSY 433
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Theories & Models.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Introduction.
Comparing the effectiveness of orthographic and phonological cues in the treatment of anomia. Lyndsey Nickels 1, Antje Lorenz 1,2, 1 Macquarie Centre for.
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition Christine P. Malone Minnesota State University Moorhead.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Speech errors cont.
Interactivity in lexical access The modularity debate.
Against formal phonology (Port and Leary).  Generative phonology assumes:  Units (phones) are discrete (not continuous, not variable)  Phonetic space.
Theories of Priming II : Types of Primes Timothy McNamara Journal of Experimental Psychology,1994 조 성 식조 성 식.
VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION. What is Word Recognition? Features, letters & word interactions Interactive Activation Model Lexical and Sublexical Approach.
Rob Hartsuiker (Ghent University) Martin Pickering & Nivja de Jong
Structural, Phonological, Semantic
Cognitive Processes in SLL and Bilinguals:
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition
Sentence Production.
Amie Fairs, Sara Bögels, Antje S. Meyer
عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition
Presentation transcript:

PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models

Uhh… It is a.. You know.. A.. Arggg. I can almost see it, it has two Syllables, I think it starts with A ….. TOT Meaning access No (little) phonological access What about syntax? Tip-of-the-tongue

Semantics Syntax grammatical category (“part of speech”) e.g. noun, verb, adjective Gender e.g. le chien, la vache; le camion, la voiture Number e.g. dog vs. dogs; trousers vs. shirt Count/mass status e.g. oats vs. flour Tip-of-the-tongue

Vigliocco et al. (1997) Subjects presented with word definitions Gender was always arbitrary If unable to retrieve word, they answered How well do you think you know the word? Guess the gender Guess the number of syllables Guess as many letters and positions as possible Report any word that comes to mind Then presented with target word Do you know this word? Is this the word you were thinking of? Tip-of-the-tongue

Vigliocco et al (1997) Scoring + TOT Both reported some correct information in questionnaire And said yes to recognition question - TOT Otherwise Vigliocco et al. (1997)

Vigliocco et al (1997) Results + TOT: 84% correct gender guess - TOT: 53% correct gender guess chance level Conclusion Subjects often know grammatical gender information even when they have no phonological information Supports split between syntax and phonology in production Vigliocco et al. (1997)

Comparing models Central questions: Are the stages discrete or cascading? Discrete: must complete before moving on Cascade: can get started as soon as some information is available Is there feedback? Top-down only Bottom up too How many levels are there?

Levelt’s model Four broad stages: Conceptualisation deciding on the message (= meaning to express) Formulation turning the message into linguistic representations Grammatical encoding (finding words and putting them together) Phonological encoding (finding sounds and putting them together) Articulation speaking (or writing or signing) Monitoring (via the comprehension system)

Network has three strata conceptual stratum lemma stratum word-form stratum Levelt’s model

Tip of tongue state when lemma is retrieved without word-form being retrieved Levelt’s model Formulation involves lexical retrieval: Semantic/syntactic content (lemma) Phonological content (word- form)

has stripesis dangerous TIGER (X) Fem. Nouncountable tigre /tigre/ /t//I//g/ Lexical concepts Lemmas Lexemes Phonemes Levelt’s model Lexicon

Conceptual stratum Conceptual stratum is not decomposed one lexical concept node for “tiger” instead, conceptual links from “tiger” to “stripes”, etc. has stripesis dangerous TIGER (X)

First, lemma activation occurs This involves activating a lemma or lemmas corresponding to the concept thus, concept TIGER activates lemma “tiger” Lexical selection Fem. Nouncountable tiger TIGER (X)

First, lemma activation occurs This involves activating a lemma or lemmas corresponding to the concept thus, concept TIGER activates lemma “tiger” Lexical selection tiger But also involves activating other lemmas TIGER also activates LION (etc.) to some extent and LION activates lemma “lion” TIGER (X)LION (X) lion

Selection is different from activation Only one lemma is selected Probability of selecting the target lemma (“tiger”) ratio of that lemma’s activation to the total activation of all lemmas (“tiger”, “lion”, etc.) Hence competition between semantically related lemmas Lemma selection tiger TIGER (X)LION (X) lion

Morpho-phonological encoding (and beyond) The lemma is now converted into a phonological representation called “word-form” (or “lexeme”) If “tiger” lemma plus plural (and noun) are activated Leads to activation of morphemes tigre and s Other processes too Stress, phonological segments, phonetics, and finally articulation /tigre/ /t//I//g/

Modularity Later processes cannot affect earlier processes No feedback between the word-form (lexemes) layer and the grammatical (lemmas) layer Also, only one lemma activates a word form If “tiger” and “lion” lemmas are activated, they compete to produce a winner at the lemma stratum Only the “winner” activates a word form The word-forms for the “losers” aren’t accessed Model’s assumptions

tiger Picture-word interference task Participants name basic objects as quickly as possible Distractor words are embedded in the object participants are instructed to ignore these words Experimental tests

Semantically related words can interfere with naming e.g., the word TIGER in a picture of a LION Basic findings tiger

However, form-related words can speed up processing e.g., the word liar in a picture of a LION Basic findings liar

Experiments manipulate timing: picture and word can be presented simultaneously time liar

Experiments manipulate timing: picture and word can be presented simultaneously liar time liar or one can slightly precede the other We draw inferences about time-course of processing

Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990) SOA (Stimulus onset asynchrony) manipulation -150 ms (word …150 ms … picture) 0 ms (i.e., synchronous presentation) +150 ms (picture …150ms …word) Auditory presentation of distractors DOT phonologically related CAT semantically related SHIP unrelated word

Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990) Auditory presentation of distractors DOT phonologically related CAT semantically related SHIP unrelated word Early Only Semantic effects

Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990) Auditory presentation of distractors DOT phonologically related CAT semantically related SHIP unrelated word Late Only Phonological effects

Early semantic inhibition Late phonological facilitation Fits with the assumption that semantic processing precedes phonological processing No overlap suggests two discrete stages in production an interactive account might find semantic and phonological effects at the same time Interpretation

Dell’s interactive account Dell (1986) presented the best-known interactive account other similar accounts exist 3 levels of representation semantics (decomposed into features) words phonemes (sounds)

Interactive because information flows “upwards” as well as “downwards” e.g., the semantic features mammal, barks, four-legs activate the word “dog” this activates the sounds /d/, /o/, /g/ these send activation back to the word level, activating words containing these sounds (e.g., “log”, “dot”) to some extent this activation is upwards (phonology to syntax) and wouldn’t occur in Levelt’s account

Mixed errors Both semantic and phonological relationship to target word Target = “cat” semantic error = “dog” phonological error = “hat” mixed error = “rat” Occur more often than predicted by modular models if you can go wrong at either stage, it would only be by chance that an error would be mixed Evidence for Dell’s model

The semantic features of dog activate lemma “cat” Some features (e.g., animate, mammalian) activate “rat” as well “cat” then activates the sounds /k/, /ae/, /t/ /ae/ and /t/ activate “rat” by feedback this confluence of activation leads to increased tendency for “rat” to be uttered Also explains the tendency for phonological errors to be real words Sounds can only feed back to words (non-words not represented) so only words can feedback to sound level Dell’s explanation

Why might interaction occur? Can’t exist just to produce errors! So what is feedback for? Perhaps because the same network is used in comprehension So feedback would be the normal comprehension route Alternatively, it simply serves to increase fluency in lemma selection advantageous to select a lemma whose phonological form is easy to find

Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990) Also looked for any evidence of a mediated priming effect hat dog DOG (X)CAT (X) cat /cat//hat/ /t//a//k//h/ Found no evidence for it

Alternatively, it simply serves to increase fluency in lemma selection advantageous to select a lemma whose phonological form is easy to find

Evidence for interactivity A number of recent experimental findings appear to support interaction under some circumstances (or at least cascading models) Peterson & Savoy (JEP:LMC, 1998) Cutting & Ferreira (JEP:LMC, 1999) Griffin & Bock (JML, 1998) Damian & Martin (JEP:LMC, 1999)

Peterson & Savoy found evidence for phonological activation of near synonyms: Participants slower to say distractor soda than unrelated distractor when naming couch Soda is related to non-selected sofa remember that Levelt et al. assume that only one lemma can be selected and hence activate a phonological form Levelt et al’s explanation: Could be erroneous selection of two lemmas?

Damian and Martin (1999) Extension of Schriefers et al.’s picture- word interference task remember that semantic inhibition occurred early, phonological facilitation occurred late (with no overlap) various methodological changes and developments focus on Experiment 3

The critical difference from Schriefers et al. is the addition of a “semantic and phonological” condition Picture of Apple peach (semantically related) apathy (phonologically related) apricot (sem & phono related) couch (unrelated) (also no-word control, always fast)

Results Relatedness-150 ms0 ms+ 150 ms Unrelated Semantic Phono S & P

Summary of findings early semantic inhibition (- 150 and 0 ms) late phonological facilitation (0 and ms) shows overlap, unlike Schriefers et al. but S & P condition didn’t show early semantic inhibition

This last finding demonstrates that semantic interference is reduced in the simultaneous presence of a phonological relationship (which should facilitate) Thus the finding appears to contradict the “discrete two-step” account of Levelt et al.

Can the two-stage account be saved? Evidence for interaction is hard to reconcile with the Levelt account however, most attempts are likely to revolve around the monitor basically, people sometimes notice a problem and screen it out Levelt argues that evidence for interaction really involves “special cases”, not directly related to normal processing

Summary Levelt et al.’s theory of word production: Strictly modular lexical access Syntactic processing precedes phonological processing Dell’s interactive account: Interaction between syntactic and phonological processing Experimental evidence is equivocal, but increasing evidence that more than one lemma may activate associated wordform

Caramazza’s alternative Caramazza and colleagues argue against the existence of the lemma node instead they propose a direct link between semantic level and lexeme syntactic information is associated with the lexeme Also assumes separate lexemes for written and spoken production This is really a different issue

Much evidence comes from patient data But also evidence from the independence of syntactic and phonological information in TOT states see discussion of Vigliocco et al. also Caramazza and Miozzo (Cognition, 1997; see also replies by Roelofs et al.)