Point of View and Theory of Mind Jill de Villiers Smith College Thomas Roeper University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Simple Sentences.
Advertisements

Social Communication Three to Six Years Old. Goal: Use words, phrases and sentences to inform, direct, ask questions and express anticipation, imagination,
STATEMENTS QUESTIONS INSTRUCTIONS
The people Look for some people. Write it down. By the water
Temper Tantrums By: Alison Anderson-Crum Early Childhood Education Lively Technical Center.
EFFECTIVE COMMUNICATION
Unit 9 Saving the earth Grammar--Inversion.
MODALS May, might, can, could. Modals give an indication of a speaker’s attitude and of the relationship between the speaker and the person spoken to.
a cura della prof.ssa Domitilla Gerini
Reporting. Introduction There are two main ways of reporting people’s words, thoughts, beliefs, etc.  Direct Speech  Indirect Speech.
UNIT 2 GIVING DIRECTIONS.
Direct and Indirect Speech. Direct Speech In direct speech, the original speaker's exact words are given and are indicated by quotation marks. "I don't.
The Simple Future The Simple Future has two different forms in English, "will" and "be going to." Although the two forms can sometimes be used interchangeably,
Created by: Ms. Brinson 
Comma Usage By Alfred Taylor 1www.booksbyalfredtaylor.com.
THEORY OF THE MIND and DEAF CHILDREN. What Does it Mean to Say Someone Has “Theory of the Mind?”
CAS LX 502 Semantics 1b. The Truth Ch. 1.
2-Deixis and distance.
Adverbs.
Parts of Speech. Eight parts of speech Nouns Verbs Adjectives Adverbs Pronouns Prepositions Conjunctions Interjections.
Montse Flores Adeva & Ana Hernández Bartolomé
Power of the Resurrection November 8. Think About It When was a time you told a true story that other people found hard to believe? Today we look at some.
Reported Speech Roll No Presented By:- Class: Ixth “A”
Phrases, Clauses, and Sentence Types
THE REPORTED SPEECH What is it? How do we use it?.
Level 1: The Parts of Speech
Direct-Indirect Speech
Adverbs. Lesson Overview What is an Adverb? How adverbs are form? Kinds of Adverbs. -adverbs of Manner -adverbs of Place -adverbs of Time -adverbs of.
Have you ever had to correct a student? What happened? How did you feel? What did you expect from the student? Do you remember when you were corrected.
Reported Speech “He said what??!!”. Two types of speech Direct speech ( דיבור ישיר ) Indirect speech ( דיבור עקיף )
Noun Clauses * A noun clause is a dependent/ subordinate clause that plays the role of a noun (i.e., name a person, a place or a thing) * Like any noun,
TO+VERB TO PLAY Infinitives. To form an infinitive, we use to+ the base form of a verb (to find, to help, to run, to be) I want to find a job. I want.
Reported Speech There are two ways of relating what a person has said: direct and indirect. In direct speech we repeat the original speaker’s exact words.
Question Forms and Reported Speech. Normal word order is used in reported questions, that is, the subject comes before the verb, and it is not necessary.
LECTURE 2: SEMANTICS IN LINGUISTICS
Chapter 16 Children, the Early Years. Enduring Understandings Unit Six Preschoolers learn through play.
Presupposition is what the speaker assumes to be the case prior to making an utterance. Entailment, which is not a pragmatic concept, is what logically.
I.
Sight Word List.
Making adverbs from adjectives To change an adjective into an adverb, simply add ‘ly’ as a surfix. However, if the adjective ends in ‘y’ change to ‘ily’.
CONDITIONAL SENTENCES
Quotation Punctuation. Rule 1 Use quotation marks to set off a direct quotation only. Examples: "When will you be here?" he asked. He asked when you will.
Into English 3 Unit 1 12th Grade Miss. Peres. N C M O A I T I M N U C O  Make up as many words as you can from these letters.  Count  Mountain  Can.
CHAPTER SIX GRAMMAR MOSAIC 2.
Sight Words.
Week 4. Working with the person sitting next to you, answer the following questions. Remember to use full sentences! » What might you buy if you had more.
Unit 3 Grammar Form & Function Level 3
Unit 6 Grammar Forms & Functions 3
NOUN CLAUSES.
1. It is one way of relating what a person has said. In direct speech we repeat the original speaker’s exact words ‘I am going to Oxford with my parents.
Verb Moods Identifying and writing them correctly.
GRAMMAR AND PUNCTUATION REVISE AND REVIEW WORD CLASSES.
Monday August 10, 2015 Students wrote an autobiographical letter.
LEXICAL EMPHASIS Profª. Flávia Cunha. LEXICAL EMPHASIS It is achieved by means of special words or phrases. Certain words tend to be emphatic because.
Moody Verbs. What is mood? In life: How you are feeling at a given time. In literature: The reader’s emotional response or the atmosphere of the story.
PRAGMATICS. SCHEDULE May 14: Yule ch. 1, 2 and 3 May 16: Yule ch. 4, 5 and 6 May 21: Yule ch. 7, 8 and 9 May 22: Seminar EXAM Thursday; May 31,
Parts of a Sentence. John swims. Dogs run. Children sing. Nouns: JohnDogsChildren.
Oxford Words
Infinitives and Gerunds. In beginning English grammar, we usually teach that : some verbs are followed by infinitives, some verbs are followed by gerunds,
This I Believe Essay Writer’s Workshop: Introductions, Juicy Details, & Conclusions 8 th ELA St. Joseph School.
---DGP Instructions--- MONDAY: Parts of Speech. Steps for Mondays 1. Find and label all nouns. Be aware of gerunds or infinitives acting as nouns. 2.
Modals of lost opportunity
PARTS OF SPEECH English, 7 th Grade. NOUNS -Person, place or thing -Person is usually a proper noun, if not….mailman, cashier -Place: veterinarian’s office,
Lesson Topic: Using Commas ,.
Parts of Speech Copy all text that is highlighted in RED in your notebook like we started in class. All other information may be copied or just reviewed.
Noun Clauses Chapter 12.
NOUN CLAUSE.
CONDITIONAL SENTENCES
Sentence Structure NINTH GRADE ENGLISH.
Noun Clauses.
Presentation transcript:

Point of View and Theory of Mind Jill de Villiers Smith College Thomas Roeper University of Massachusetts, Amherst

Definitions What do we mean by Point of View? In using language, it is necessary to appreciate the different perspectives of speaker and hearer. Point of View is entailed in each of: – personal reference(I/you) – spatial terms (here/there, infront/behind) – emotional attitudes (desire verbs, attitudinal adjectives and adverbs) –mental state verbs (think, know) –in complex interactions among these domains

Where does it begin? By the end of the first year of life, normally developing infants –Respond to another’s eye gaze by looking at the same object –Respond to pointing by following the point and gaze of the other –Assume intentionality e.g. for animate but not inanimate entities in reaching (Woodward) –Take turns both vocally and in play behavior –Make “proto-requests” for help By one year of age, infants attend to other people’s behavior and treat others as sources of information or help.

Deixis Pronouns: “I” and “you” are attached to speaker and hearer, not to a fixed referent. Very early, children mistake these for names and say e.g. “Pick you up” (even signers-Pettito,1987) This stage rapidly disappears, usually by 2.5 years, but is protracted in children with autism. This/that: These introduce the additional difficulty of size of space: “ this hotel”, “this pencil”, “this comma” - the space contrast shifts! With a defined barrier, children can attend to the contrast at 3 years.

Deixis, continued Here/there: Like “this/that”, they not only switch with speaker, but the space can zoom out or in in reference. With a barrier to define the difference, 3 year olds can handle the contrast in comprehension and production (de Villiers & de Villiers, 1974). Question: Do children in fact attend to the speaker’s PoV to accomplish this, or do they just have a “switch reference” possibility? Spanish, Japanese have a three way contrast: here, there, and “over there” i.e. away from BOTH. Perhaps this is the one that requires true perspective taking? ( Ueda, p.c. 2004)

Desire Want: the verb is among the first fifty words in MacArthur inventory. It’s mostly in reference to the child’s own desires However, children by 2 understand that others may like things they don’t like (Rechapoli & Gopnik, 1997) At least, they give someone a food that that person has expressed pleasure towards, even when they hate it. And by 3, they refer not only to I want but also you want/he wants etc. (Bartsch & Wellman. 1995)

Desire, continued At 3.5, they judge that someone wanted something even though they did not achieve it (Witt, 2000). They appreciate that the person wanted it under a certain description e.g. Cookie Monster reaches for what he thinks is a cookie but then discovers it’s a ball: Children at 3-4 years agree that he didn’t want the ball, he wanted a cookie. So by 4, they understand that referents are linked to their subjects, in the case of desire. The actual is irrelevant.

Mental verbs The appearance of mental verbs like think, know is early (3 years) but often stereotyped e.g. “I don’t know” or “I think I can”. There are sporadic real uses e.g. The breakthrough comes around age four years when children can understand that mental verbs can take a whole sentence in their scope (a complement) e.g Mom thought that the shampoo was the toothpaste And the embedded sentence can be FALSE from the child’s Point of View, but TRUE for Mom.

Mental verbs Once the child has this capacity, he can represent two worlds: his own, and someone else’s mental world. Verbs of communication like say, tell, provide the bootstrap for figuring out complements with mental verbs (de Villiers, 1995;2004). The language paves the way for reasoning about others’ mental states: False Belief understanding. Language in this domain seems to drive Theory of Mind rather then vice versa.

Mental verbs How do we know? A) longitudinal studies with typically developing children (de Villiers & Pyers, 2002) B) Studies of language delayed deaf children, who are equivalently delayed in ToM reasoning (de Villiers & de Villiers, 2000;2003) C) Studies of language disordered children (de Villiers, Burns & Pearson, 2003) D) Training studies that teach normally developing children communication verbs with complements and improve ToM reasoning (Hale & Tager- Flusberg, 2003;Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003)

Reference under embedding Referential opacity Children still have to figure out the right Point of View for the nouns in the embedded clause. For example, suppose Mom mistakes the shampoo for toothpaste. We know she is holding shampoo. Does Mom think she is holding shampoo? 4 and 5 year olds often say “yes” even after they have appreciated her mistake. They must learn that they need to use her word inside the complement, despite what they know the object to be. They may have the rules for referring under their own control, but not necessarily see other’s mistakes in usage for a while (de Villiers, 2004).

Model of PoV and ToM Early ToM: gaze shared attention, pointing Differentiation of self and other Appreciation of switch in perspective Early language: names, actions, social routines I/you. Here/there This/that Articles a/the He says that p He thinks that p He knows that p Ability to represent two different worlds, hence, false beliefs, content of other minds Appreciation of different tastes, likes, attitudes I want, you want, he wants Yucky, nice, scary Deixis, pronouns, adjectives, reference and tense markers under embedding, Plus multiple embedding… Increasing capacity for abstract reasoning about complex human events ToM PoV

Some subtleties Consider the way these different elements interact in the language. These cases represent the challenge to the language learning child and much remains to be understood about them.

Articles A => the may just require a switch w/o PoV. Bridge: A boy arrived. The boy was tall I bought a car. The muffler fell off. Which uses require POV? POV of speaker emerges: Did the baby eat the best cheese? Speaker or Speaker/Hearer POV Variable POV: –You each have a pencil--> pick up the pencil = must be pencil of each of you (not neighbor’s!)

Articles allow Temporal Displacement: 1. “All the sick children are well” 2. The professors were radicals in the sixties = current professors, who were students then 3. There were radical professors in the sixties = No article, therefore professors then Bock and Romero (2000): Mice got hot, then went swimming in a cool pool Were the hot mice in the pool => yes Were there hot mice in the pool => no Children yrs 17 No article variation 12 Adult pattern Articles and Time

Articles and Speaker -PoV Imagine Sarah tells Sam that she is going to buy a new coat. Sam comes home and finds a coat lying on the sofa. In fact it is Sarah’s old coat, but Sam thinks it is a new one. One can say: Sam thought the old coat was the new coat. which is paradoxical, on the surface! But “the old coat” is the speaker’s PoV And “the new coat” is Sam’s PoV.

Adjectives=> Subject or Speaker Mary likes yucky clothes = Speaker-POV = Speaker thinks they are yucky Do children grasp POV contrast? Do they see yucky as Mary’s view too? Susan’s darn beloved dog Speaker Subject *beloved darn dog Universal Grammar: Speaker adjective is on outside

Adverbs Unfortunately, definitely, sure, really, maybe maybe = it may be = General POV “Cause you're really tired” (Nina, 2;9.26) = very or it is real that… Verbal modifier: “Yeah, really she didn't.” (Nina, 3;2.12) “Is Paul really just a baby?” (Adam, 3;4.18)

Subject-adjectives Meanly, anxiously, eagerly 1. Wisely, John told the truth = John was wise Speaker Adverbs: honestly, truthfully, wisely, luckily 2. Honestly, Bill lied *it was honest that Bill lied [I am honest] Bill lied = Speaker POV Verbuk (2003): Children below 5yrs Fail to reliably construe Speaker POV

Pronouns under embedding Quotation; John said “I can do it” I = subject John said that I can do it I = Speaker Indefinite embedding: John said that Bill said that Susan believed that I can do it I = Speaker John believes that I made you invite him

Bart (speaker): One day we were going for a bike ride. On this bicycle. And you know bike rides can be quite dangerous. So, I decided to wear a helmet. And Deanne? She decided to wear these big blue gloves. But we weren’t all that sure whether that was safe enough and so Deanne was going to ask Daddy. But Daddy was at work. So, she had to call him and ask him. Deanne asked Daddy: “Can I ride a bike with my blue gloves? And can Bart ride a bike with a helmet? Bart : “How did Deanne ask can I ride a bike?” Hollebrandse (2000): Quotation

If Quote: How did Deanne ask “Can I ride a bike?” => on the phone Bart speaking: How did Deanne ask can I ride a bike => I = Bart => with a helmet Children: “with blue gloves” => failure to shift POV 60% 3yr olds => long-distance = ride a bike with helmet 20% 6yr olds => short-distance General Conclusion: Children do not understand POV shift with quotation immediately Results

Navajo and Arabic: (Speas, Abdul-Karim) Navajo: John thought that I can do it I = John Arabic: She said that I (fem) can, I (masc) think, do it I (fem) = Subject she I (masc) = Speaker Challenge: all children must be able to test if English might be Navajo or Arabic Time expressions in English: Yesterday John said he did it two days ago. = two days ago = Speaker = three days ago = Subject Language variation

Deixis under embedding John said yesterday on the phone “can you send him to me here now” What did he say? He said could I send you to him there then

Tanz (1978) Ask John what color he thinks my eyes are “What color do you think his eyes are?” Children 4yrs can make joint POV shift Conclusion: all POV elements are linked in a POV-CHAIN PoV chain