Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a. 05-06 prof S. Presciuttini 1. Heritability of a trait The most basic question to be asked about a quantitative trait.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
15 The Genetic Basis of Complex Inheritance
Advertisements

Basic Mendelian Principles
Quantitative traits.
Qualitative and Quantitative traits
Pleiotropy: One gene influences more than one trait
Chapter 6: Quantitative traits, breeding value and heritability Quantitative traits Phenotypic and genotypic values Breeding value Dominance deviation.
Pinker, S.R. (2002). The Blank Slate. New York: Viking. Children Pinker Ch. 19 Heather Steffani, Lindsey Stevenson, and Fatima Coley.
Genesis 25: And when her days to be delivered were fulfilled, behold, there were twins in her womb. 25 And the first came out red, all over like.
Quantitative genetics
Chapter 7 Quantitative Genetics Read Chapter 7 sections 7.1 and 7.2. [You should read 7.3 and 7.4 to deepen your understanding of the topic, but I will.
Patterns of inheritance
Quantitative Genetics Up until now, we have dealt with characters (actually genotypes) controlled by a single locus, with only two alleles: Discrete Variation.
1 15 The Genetic Basis of Complex Inheritance. 2 Multifactorial Traits Multifactorial traits are determined by multiple genetic and environmental factors.
The Inheritance of Complex Traits
Quantitative Genetics Theoretical justification Estimation of heritability –Family studies –Response to selection –Inbred strain comparisons Quantitative.
Transmission Genetics: Heritage from Mendel 2. Mendel’s Genetics Experimental tool: garden pea Outcome of genetic cross is independent of whether the.
The Inheritance of Complex Traits
Genetics Human Genome Behavioral Genetics Family Studies Twin Studies
Lecture 5 Artificial Selection R = h 2 S. Applications of Artificial Selection Applications in agriculture and forestry Creation of model systems of human.
Quantitative Genetics
Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 1. Mendel’s dihybrid crosses Mendel went on to analyze the descendants of pure lines that.
Introducing Psychology
NORMAL DISTRIBUTIONS OF PHENOTYPES Mice Fruit Flies In:Introduction to Quantitative Genetics Falconer & Mackay 1996.
Quantitative Genetics
Mendel and the Idea of the Gene
Review Session Monday, November 8 Shantz 242 E (the usual place) 5:00-7:00 PM I’ll answer questions on my material, then Chad will answer questions on.
Chapter 7: Genes and Inheritance Family resemblance: how traits are inherited Lectures by Mark Manteuffel, St. Louis Community College.
ConceptS and Connections
Broad-Sense Heritability Index
Multifactorial Traits
Chapter 5 Characterizing Genetic Diversity: Quantitative Variation Quantitative (metric or polygenic) characters of Most concern to conservation biology.
General Psychology. Scripture Matthew 5: 9 Blessed are the peacemakers, for they will be called children of God.. Those who strive to prevent contention,
Quantitative Genetics
1 PSYCHOLOGY (8th Edition, in Modules) David Myers.
1 Phenotypic Variation Variation of a trait can be separated into genetic and environmental components Genotypic variance  g 2 = variation in phenotype.
Mendelian Patterns of Inheritance Chapter 9. Introduction Gazelle always produce baby gazelles, not bluebirds.
PBG 650 Advanced Plant Breeding
Trait evolution Up until now, we focused on microevolution – the forces that change allele and genotype frequencies in a population This portion of the.
Humans have bred dogs to produce tremendous variety. But a new study reports that the physical variance among dog breeds is determined by differences.
PATTERNS OF INHERITANCE CAPTER 10. Pre-Mendel’s Theories  Blending Hypothesis: when parents with different traits have offspring, this will always show.
Mendel’s Legacy Genetics is everywhere these days – and it will continue as a dominant force in biology and society for decades to come. Wouldn’t it be.
Biology 15.2 How Populations Evolve How Populations Evolve.
Heredity – Chapter 4 Mendelian Genetics, Monohybrid and Dihybrid Crosses and Beyond Mendel’s Laws.
Chapter 22 - Quantitative genetics: Traits with a continuous distribution of phenotypes are called continuous traits (e.g., height, weight, growth rate,
Lecture Outline What is Development? Themes/Issues in Developmental Psychology Developmental Systems Theories.
NORMAL DISTRIBUTIONS OF PHENOTYPES Mice Fruit Flies In:Introduction to Quantitative Genetics Falconer & Mackay 1996.
Genetics and Behavior. Behavior Geneticists study our differences and weigh the relative effects of heredity and environment.
Genetics Intro. Phenotype Observable, Physical traits (ear shape, petal color) these are expressed biologically. –Offspring usually have a phenotype similar.
IV. Variation in Quantitative Traits A. Quantitative Effects.
Nature & Nurture Dr. Carolyn R. Fallahi. Nature-Nurture  Which is more important?  The Diathesis stress model  We figure out the relative amount of.
STT2073 Plant Breeding and Improvement. Quality vs Quantity Quality: Appearance of fruit/plant/seed – size, colour – flavour, taste, texture – shelflife.
Genetics And Heredity Introduction. Gregor Mendel Made many observations that led to the model of inheritance that we use today.
Active Lecture PowerPoint ® Presentation for Essentials of Genetics Seventh Edition Klug, Cummings, Spencer, Palladino Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education,
NORMAL DISTRIBUTIONS OF PHENOTYPES
Evolution by Natural Selection as a Syllogism
NORMAL DISTRIBUTIONS OF PHENOTYPES
Genetics: Analysis and Principles
Quantitative Variation
Spring 2009: Section 5 – Lecture 1
Intelligence: The Dynamics of Intelligence
Genetics vs. Environment
Studying Twins and Adoptions Can Help Assess the Importance of Genes and Environment Genetics.
Pi = Gi + Ei Pi = pi - p Gi = gi - g Ei = ei - e _ _ _ Phenotype
Chapter 7 Beyond alleles: Quantitative Genetics
A Lesson From the Zuni Indians: Heritability in Perspective
TO WHAT EXTENT DOES GENETIC INHERITANCE INFLUENCE BEHAVIOR?
63.1 – Discuss the evidence for a genetic influence on intelligence and explain what is meant by heritability. Nature vs. Nurture and Intelligence Early.
Nature vs. Nurture in IQ Are differences between people due to environmental or genetic differences? Misunderstanding the question “Is a person’s intelligence.
Presentation transcript:

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 1. Heritability of a trait The most basic question to be asked about a quantitative trait is whether the observed variation in the character is influenced by genes at all. It is important to note that this is not the same as asking whether genes play any role in the character's development. The most basic question to be asked about a quantitative trait is whether the observed variation in the character is influenced by genes at all. It is important to note that this is not the same as asking whether genes play any role in the character's development. Gene-mediated developmental processes lie at the base of every character, but variation from individual to individual is not necessarily the result of genetic variation. Gene-mediated developmental processes lie at the base of every character, but variation from individual to individual is not necessarily the result of genetic variation.  For example, the possibility of speaking any language at all depends critically on the structures of the central nervous system as well as of the vocal cords, tongue, mouth, and ears, which depend in turn on the nature of the human genome. There is no environment in which cows will speak. But, although the particular language that is spoken by humans varies from nation to nation, that variation is totally nongenetic.

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 2. Familiality and heritability In principle, it is easy to determine whether any genetic variation influences the phenotypic variation among organisms for a particular trait. If genes are involved, then (on average) biological relatives should resemble one another more than unrelated individuals do. In principle, it is easy to determine whether any genetic variation influences the phenotypic variation among organisms for a particular trait. If genes are involved, then (on average) biological relatives should resemble one another more than unrelated individuals do. This resemblance would be seen as a positive correlation between parents and offspring or between siblings (offspring of the same parents). Parents who are larger than the average would have offspring who are larger than the average; the more seeds that a plant produces, the more seeds that its siblings would produce. Such correlations between relatives, however, are evidence for genetic variation only if the relatives do not share common environments more than nonrelatives do. It is absolutely fundamental to distinguish familiality from heritability. Traits are familial if members of the same family share them, for whatever reason. Traits are heritable only if the similarity arises from shared genotypes. This resemblance would be seen as a positive correlation between parents and offspring or between siblings (offspring of the same parents). Parents who are larger than the average would have offspring who are larger than the average; the more seeds that a plant produces, the more seeds that its siblings would produce. Such correlations between relatives, however, are evidence for genetic variation only if the relatives do not share common environments more than nonrelatives do. It is absolutely fundamental to distinguish familiality from heritability. Traits are familial if members of the same family share them, for whatever reason. Traits are heritable only if the similarity arises from shared genotypes.

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 3. Phenotypic similarity between relatives In experimental organisms, there is no problem in separating environmental from genetic similarities. In experimental organisms, there is no problem in separating environmental from genetic similarities.  The offspring of a cow producing milk at a high rate and the offspring of a cow producing milk at a low rate can be raised together in the same environment to see whether, despite the environmental similarity, each resembles its own parent. In natural populations, and especially in humans, this is difficult to do. Because of the nature of human societies, members of the same family not only share genes, but also have similar environments. Thus, the observation of simple familiality of a trait is genetically uninterpretable. In natural populations, and especially in humans, this is difficult to do. Because of the nature of human societies, members of the same family not only share genes, but also have similar environments. Thus, the observation of simple familiality of a trait is genetically uninterpretable.  In general, people who speak Hungarian have Hungarian-speaking parents and people who speak Japanese have Japanese-speaking parents. Yet the massive experience of immigration to different countries has demonstrated that these linguistic differences, although familial, are nongenetic. The highest correlations between parents and offspring for any social traits are often those for political party and religious sect, but they are not heritable. However, the distinction between familiality and heredity is not always so obvious. The highest correlations between parents and offspring for any social traits are often those for political party and religious sect, but they are not heritable. However, the distinction between familiality and heredity is not always so obvious.  For example, The Public Health Commission, which originally studied the vitamin deficiency disease pellagra in the southern United States in 1910, came to the conclusion that it was genetic because it ran in families.

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 4. Heritability of behavioral traits Personality traits, temperament, and cognitive performance (including IQ scores), as well as a whole variety of behaviors such as alcoholism and of mental disorders such as schizophrenia, have been the subject of heritability studies in human populations. Many show familiality. Personality traits, temperament, and cognitive performance (including IQ scores), as well as a whole variety of behaviors such as alcoholism and of mental disorders such as schizophrenia, have been the subject of heritability studies in human populations. Many show familiality. There is indeed a positive correlation between the IQ scores of parents and the scores of their children (the correlation is about 0.5 in white American families), but the correlation does not distinguish familiality from heritability. There is indeed a positive correlation between the IQ scores of parents and the scores of their children (the correlation is about 0.5 in white American families), but the correlation does not distinguish familiality from heritability. To make that distinction requires that the environmental correlation between parents and children be broken, so adoption studies are common. Because it is difficult to randomize the environments, even in cases of adoption, evidence of heritability for human personality and behavior traits remains equivocal despite the very large number of studies that exist. To make that distinction requires that the environmental correlation between parents and children be broken, so adoption studies are common. Because it is difficult to randomize the environments, even in cases of adoption, evidence of heritability for human personality and behavior traits remains equivocal despite the very large number of studies that exist. Prejudices about the causes of human differences are widespread and deep, and, as a result, the canons of evidence adhered to in studies of the heritability of IQ, for example, have been much more lax than in studies of milk yield in cows. Prejudices about the causes of human differences are widespread and deep, and, as a result, the canons of evidence adhered to in studies of the heritability of IQ, for example, have been much more lax than in studies of milk yield in cows.

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 5. Quantifying Heritability If a trait is shown to have some heritability in a population, then it is possible to quantify the degree of heritability. We have seen that the variation between phenotypes in a population arises from two sources. If a trait is shown to have some heritability in a population, then it is possible to quantify the degree of heritability. We have seen that the variation between phenotypes in a population arises from two sources. First, there are average differences between the genotypes; second, each genotype exhibits phenotypic variance because of environmental variation. The total phenotypic variance of the population V P can then be broken into two parts: the variance between genotypic means V G and the remaining variance V E. The former is called the genetic variance, and the latter is called the environmental variance First, there are average differences between the genotypes; second, each genotype exhibits phenotypic variance because of environmental variation. The total phenotypic variance of the population V P can then be broken into two parts: the variance between genotypic means V G and the remaining variance V E. The former is called the genetic variance, and the latter is called the environmental variance The degree of heritability can be defined as the part of the total variance that is due to genetic variance: The degree of heritability can be defined as the part of the total variance that is due to genetic variance: H 2 = V G / V P = V G /(V G + V E ) H 2, so defined, is called the broad heritability of the character. H 2, so defined, is called the broad heritability of the character.

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 6. The Meaning of H 2 Attention to the problems of estimating broad heritability distracts from the deeper questions about the meaning of the ratio when it can be estimated. Despite its widespread use as a measure of how “important” genes are in influencing a trait, H 2 actually has a special and limited meaning. Attention to the problems of estimating broad heritability distracts from the deeper questions about the meaning of the ratio when it can be estimated. Despite its widespread use as a measure of how “important” genes are in influencing a trait, H 2 actually has a special and limited meaning. There are two conclusions that can be drawn from a properly designed heritability study. There are two conclusions that can be drawn from a properly designed heritability study.  First, if there is a nonzero heritability, then, in the population measured and in the environments in which the organisms have developed, genetic differences have influenced the variation between individuals, so genetic differences do matter to the trait. This is not a trivial finding and is a first step in a more detailed investigation of the role of genes.  Second, the value of the H 2 provides a limited prediction of the effect of environmental modification under particular circumstances. If all the relevant environmental variation is eliminated and the new constant environment is the same as the mean environment in the original population, then H 2 estimates how much phenotypic variation will still be present. So, if the heritability of performance on an IQ test were, say, 0.4, then, if all children had the same developmental and social environment as the “average child,” about 60 percent of the variation in IQ test performance would disappear and 40 percent would remain.

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 7. Additive and Dominance Variance Knowledge of the broad heritability (H2) of a trait in a population is not very useful in itself, but a finer subdivision of phenotypic variance can provide important information for plant and animal breeders. The genetic variation and the environmental variation can themselves each be further subdivided to provide information about gene action and the possibility of shaping the genetic composition of a population. Knowledge of the broad heritability (H2) of a trait in a population is not very useful in itself, but a finer subdivision of phenotypic variance can provide important information for plant and animal breeders. The genetic variation and the environmental variation can themselves each be further subdivided to provide information about gene action and the possibility of shaping the genetic composition of a population. When a heterozygote A/a is not exactly intermediate in phenotype between the two homozygotes A/A and a/a, then all the genetic variation for phenotype cannot be accounted for by differences in the average effect in the population of A and a alleles. Some of the genetic variance is the result of the deviation of the heterozygote from the phenotype expected from a simple averaging of the two allelic effects. The total genetic variance in the population can then be subdivided into additive genetic variation, V A the variance that arises because there is an average difference between the carriers of a alleles and the carriers of A alleles, and a component called the dominance variance V D, which results from the fact that heterozygotes are not exactly intermediate between the monozygotes. Thus When a heterozygote A/a is not exactly intermediate in phenotype between the two homozygotes A/A and a/a, then all the genetic variation for phenotype cannot be accounted for by differences in the average effect in the population of A and a alleles. Some of the genetic variance is the result of the deviation of the heterozygote from the phenotype expected from a simple averaging of the two allelic effects. The total genetic variance in the population can then be subdivided into additive genetic variation, V A the variance that arises because there is an average difference between the carriers of a alleles and the carriers of A alleles, and a component called the dominance variance V D, which results from the fact that heterozygotes are not exactly intermediate between the monozygotes. Thus V G = V A + V D

Genetica per Scienze Naturali a.a prof S. Presciuttini 8. Heritability in the narrow sense Thus, we define a new kind of heritability, the heritability in the narrow sense (h 2 ), as Thus, we define a new kind of heritability, the heritability in the narrow sense (h 2 ), as h 2 = V A / V P = V A /(V A + V D + V E ) h 2 = V A / V P = V A /(V A + V D + V E ) It is this heritability, that is useful in determining whether a program of selective breeding will succeed in changing the population. The greater the h 2 is, the greater the difference is between selected parents and the population as a whole, which will be preserved in the offspring of the selected parents. It is this heritability, that is useful in determining whether a program of selective breeding will succeed in changing the population. The greater the h 2 is, the greater the difference is between selected parents and the population as a whole, which will be preserved in the offspring of the selected parents. Even though h 2 is a number that applies only to a particular population and a given set of environments, it is still of great practical importance to breeders. Even though h 2 is a number that applies only to a particular population and a given set of environments, it is still of great practical importance to breeders.  A poultry geneticist interested in increasing, say, growth rate is not concerned with the genetic variance over all possible flocks and all environmental distributions. Given two possible flocks, the question becomes: can a selection scheme be devised to increase growth rate and, if so, how fast? If one flock has a lot of genetic variance and another only a little, the breeder will choose the former to carry out selection. If the heritability in the chosen flock is very high, then the mean of the population will respond quickly to the selection imposed, because most of the superiority of the selected parents will appear in the offspring. The higher h 2 is, the higher the parent–offspring correlation is. If, on the other hand, h 2 is low, then only a small fraction of the increased growth rate of the selected parents will appear in the next generation.