Affectionate Touching and Intentional Communication in 9, 12, and 18 Month-Old Deaf Infants with Deaf and Hearing Mothers By: Sherri Ewing Introduction.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Authors: Marilyn Sass-Lehrer, Donna Mertens, and Kathryn Meadow-Orlans To contact the author for permission to use this PowerPoint, please marilyn.sass-
Advertisements

Age of Intended Audience: 18 and up
Acknowledgments Research Mentor: Dr. Paul Silverman Funding: Bridges to the Baccalaureate- The University of Montana Other: Sarah Lawlay Introduction Parents.
The Self-Fulfilling Prophecy of Adolescent Social Expectations. Emily L. Loeb, Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M. Schad, & Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia.
Attachment and Close Relationships: A Life Span Perspective Levitt, M. J. (1991). Attachment and close relationships: A life-span perspective. In J. L.
Acknowledgments This study was funded in part by a grant from the National Institutes of Health-Institute on Deafness and other Communicative Disorders.
Higher levels of parental depressive symptoms were related to higher levels of parental guilt induction (  =.14, p
Results – effects of having a target named Comparing known & unknown targets (“name” condition) with known & unknown pictures (“look” condition) In other.
Emotional Processes Involved in Children's Adjustment to Interparental Conflict: An Attachment Perspective Sarah Raynor, Stacy Braun, and John Grych Marquette.
Alex Miller Summer Research Assistant Brown Center for Children at Risk.
The Relationship Between Effective Engagement and Developmental Age in Children with Autism Abby Ramser Lisa Ruble, PhD.
Acknowledgments Research Mentor: Dr. Linda EagleHeart-Thomas Funding: The Bridges to the Baccalaureate Program, The University of Montana Special Thanks.
Pacifier Use and Speech Sound Development Tamara Nagoda, M.S. 1 & Peter Flipsen Jr., Ph.D. 2 1 Idaho State University, Meridian, ID 2 Pacific University,
Students With Multiple Disabilities Harold Johnson Michigan State University.
CSD 5400 REHABILITATION PROCEDURES FOR THE HARD OF HEARING Language and Speech of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Characteristics and Concerns Language Acquisition.
V v Do Older Parents have More Self-Regulated Children? Kevin G. Harper, Megan M. McClelland, Ph.D. Oregon State University COLLEGE OF PUBLIC HEALTH AND.
Maternal Scaffolding of Play and Thinking in the Infant/Toddler Period Relationships to Later IQ.
Ten Year Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Mothers and their Children Catholic Family Service of Calgary Louise Dean Centre Holly Charles & Brenda Simpson.
Discussion Milieu Teaching Strategies are used to increase both verbal and nonverbal communication interactions of children with language delays. Parents.
Students’ Understanding of Human Nature: An Analogical Approach R. BROCK FROST AND ERIC AMSEL Weber State University Introduction University students enter.
Janet R. Jamieson Susan Lane
Laura D. Pittman, Adeya Richmond, Jillian M. Wickery, Suzanne G. Venteau, Danielle M. Baran, & Marcos D. López Parenting and School Readiness among Low-Income.
R ECEPTIVE VOCABULARY DEVELOPMENT IN CHILDREN WITH COCHLEAR IMPLANTS : Achievement in an intensive, auditory-oral educational setting Heather Hayes, Ann.
Participant Information for CHILD2 CHILD1 Note: NT=sounds not taught in intervention. CHILD1 had 1031 total errors. The majority of CHILD1’s errors were.
High levels of anxiety negatively influence children’s academic performance (Mazzone, Ducci, Scoto, Passaniti, D'Arrigo, & Vitiello, 2007). Social anxiety.
Infant Directed Speech with infants at risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder Jean Quigley School of Psychology Trinity College Dublin Funded by IRCHSS.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Use of Signed Communication Among Parents of Cochlear- Implanted Children with Additional Disabilities.
RESULTS INTRODUCTION Laurentian_University.svgLaurentian_University.svg‎ (SVG file, nominally 500 × 87 pixels, file size: 57 KB) Comparison of the ASQ.
Memory in Infants  How do you study memory in an infant?  By using the orienting response & habituation  Orienting Response = tendency to respond to.
CONCLUSION There was a significant main effect of dyadic gender composition on dyadic tie strength, such that same-gender dyads demonstrated stronger ties.
Can Peer Pressure Be A Good Thing? Megan M. Schad, Meredyth A. Evans, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
ANH KIEU AND ASHLEY NGUYEN UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO Bilingualism and Sibling Relationships.
Dads Do Matter: Adolescents’ Relationships With Dads Predicting Later Rejection Sensitivity I would like to thank the William T. Grant Foundation, Spencer.
Gottman’s Social- Psychophysiological Research Protocol Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally.
The Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Bimodal and Unimodal Social Events Across Infancy Lorraine E. Bahrick, Barbara M. Sorondo, Irina Castellanos,
Youth violence exposure, adolescent delinquency and anxiety, and the potential mediating role of sleep problems during middle childhood Chelsea M. Weaver.
James M. Hadder, B.S., Christopher S. Immel, B.A., Michael M. Knepp, M.S., Russell T. Jones, Ph.D., Thomas H. Ollendick, Ph.D. INTRODUCTION METHODOLOGY.
Early Adolescence Social Withdrawal as a Predictor of Late Adolescence Autonomy and Relatedness with Romantic Partners. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M. Schad,
Test Equity Assessment and Evaluation of Individuals who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing Mary Morrison Chris Skoczynski
 By preschool age, boys and girls show marked differences on a number of emotional, social, and behavioral outcomes (Ruble et al., 2006). Some gender.
Parents' Marital Functioning and the Development of Adolescent Romantic Relationships Amanda L. Hare, F. Christy McFarland, & Joseph P. Allen University.
Childcare Mckim et al., 1999 Studied effects of childcare on attachment Participants: Families with infants between 2 and 30 months Visited homes 2-3 weeks.
The Role of Intersensory Redundancy in the Typical Development of Social Orienting Across Infancy: A New Hypothesis for Autism Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Nonsocial Events Across Infancy: Effects of Unimodal versus Bimodal Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Integrated Mother-Premature Infant Intervention and Mother-Infant Interaction at 6-weeks Corrected Age Rosemary White-Traut, PhD, RN, FAAN Kathleen Norr,
Family-School Collaboration Lara Pascoe February 10, 2011 Dr. Coleman.
Results Time 2 (Age 18-20) Target teen and their romantic partner engaged in an 8 minute hypothetical disagreement task interaction. Hostile, relationship-undermining.
Human infant sleeping locations as determined by adolescent and adult primiparous mothers K.P. Klingaman, L.E. Volpe, and J.J. McKenna University of Notre.
We would like to thank the William T. Grant Foundation, Spencer Foundation, and National Institute of Mental Health for funding provided to Joseph Allen,
Marital Satisfaction and Consensus: Links to the Development of Behavioral Social Functioning in Early Adolescence L. Wrenn Thompson Jessica Meyer Joseph.
Video-Modeling with Feedback KELSIE BURKHARD SEDP 651: RESEARCH IN ACTION VIRGINIA COMMONWEALTH UNIVERSITY.
From Managing Emotions to Improving Relationships: Higher Quality Best Friendships Predicted from Earlier Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M.
Early Shyness and School-Age Internalizing Symptoms: Mediation by Emotion Regulation Xin Feng 1, Daniel S. Shaw 2, and Kristin L. Moilanen 3 1 The Ohio.
Associations between Maternal Postnatal Depression and Infants’ Social Learning Abilities Oliver Perra, Rebecca Phillips, Rhiannon Fyfield, Cerith Waters,
menu Description Autism is a developmental disability that people can not identify from the physical appearance of autistics. Individuals who suffer.
CLS July EYE GAZE IN TURNTAKING IN SIGN LANGUAGE INTERACTION Anne Baker & Beppie van den Bogaerde.
Cognitive Development in Infancy and Childhood: information Processing
Discussion Results Introduction From Lateral to Leader: A Study of Preschoolers’ Relationships with Peers Erin Podgorski & Dr. Carin L. Neitzel, The University.
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
1 A Comparison of Motor Delays in Young Children: Autism Spectrum Disorder, Developmental Delay, and Developmental Concerns Beth Provost, Brian R. Lopez,
Can Pretty People Have Their Cake and Eat it Too? Positive and Negative Effects of Physical Attractiveness. Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Joanna M.
Preschool-Age, Low-Income, Hispanic Children; Relations Between Parent-Child Interactions and Child Language Development in English and Spanish Reina Izabel.
Influence of Baseline Physiology of Behaviorally Inhibited Preschool Children Involved in Early Intervention Samantha Hamburger, Matthew Barstead, Kelly.
Introduction Methods Discussion Hypotheses Results References
A Study of Vocalization and Mother-Infant Interactions on the Emergence of Sex-Related Differences During Infancy Carmen Jimenez-Robbins, Christelle Ngnoumen,
Consonant variegations in first words: Infants’ actual productions of
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Rudiments of the DMM Patricia M. Crittenden.
Presentation transcript:

Affectionate Touching and Intentional Communication in 9, 12, and 18 Month-Old Deaf Infants with Deaf and Hearing Mothers By: Sherri Ewing Introduction Deaf mothers use more touching to communicate in general than hearing mothers (Mohay, 2000). Parent-Infant Dyads with a mismatched communication system (i.e., deaf parents with hearing children and hearing parents with deaf children) tend to have difficulty interacting effectively (Mohay, 2000). In the current study, we were interested in how parents use affectionate touch and intentional communication with infants. Scores on the Infant-Caregiver Interaction Scale (ICIS) for Intentional Communication and Affectionate Touching were compared. It was predicted that Deaf mothers with Deaf infants (Dd) would score higher than Hearing mothers with Deaf infants (Hd) on both measures with their infants. Methods Participants Participants Video tapes were analyzed for 30 mother-infant dyads from an archival data set retrieved from the Gallaudet Infancy Study (Meadow-Orlans et al., 2004). This was a longitudinal study conducted at Gallaudet University in Washington, D.C. Materials Infant-Caregiver Interaction Scale (Munson & Odom, 1995). The ICIS is an unpublished scale developed to assess play and feeding interactions between parents and infants. We used two dimensions from the ICIS (Communicative Intent and Affectionate Touching). Discussion Overall, Dd dyads did appear to use affective touching and intentional communication more than Hd dyads at all ages. These findings were consistent with Mohay (2000). These types of interaction patterns are important to identify and understand in order to help mismatched dyads communicate more effectively. Abstract Researchers know that deaf mothers use more touching to communicate in general than hearing mothers (Mohay, 2000). In the current study, we were interested in how parents engage in affectionate touching and intentional communication with their infants. Video tapes were analyzed for 30 mother-infant dyads from an archival data set, using two dimensions from the Infant-Caregiver Interaction Scale (Munson & Odom, 1995). Deaf mothers were rated slightly higher on average than Hearing mothers on affectionate touching and intentional communication when interacting with their infants at 9, 12, & 18 mos. These interaction patterns are important to identify and understand in order to better help mismatched dyads communicate more effectively. Results ( (See figures 1 and 2) Mothers Mothers: Deaf mothers were rated slightly higher on average than Hearing mothers on affectionate touching and intentional communication at each age. Mothers communicated more than infants over all the ages. The Deaf mothers used equal amounts of touching at all ages. Hearing mothers communicated more than infants at each age. Infants Infants: The Deaf infants attempted to communicate with their Deaf mothers more at 18 mos. than 9 & 12 mos. Deaf infants at 12 mos. used more affectionate touching than at 9 & 18 mos. The Deaf infants attempted to communicate with their Hearing mothers more at 18 mos. than at 9 & 12 mos. The Deaf infants used an equal amount of touching at all ages. Figure 1 Acknowledgements The author would like to thank the people who allowed their experiences to be video taped. Also, a huge thank you to Dr. Lynne Koester, Eve Lahti-Harper, The Bridges Program, and The University of Montana. References Brazelton, T. B., & Greenspan, S. (2000). Our window to the future. Newsweek Special Issue, Meadow-Orlans, K. P., Spencer, P. E., & Koester, L. S. (2004). The world of deaf infants: A longitudinal study. New York: Oxford. Mohay, H. (2000). Language in sight: Mothers’ strategies for making language visually accessible to deaf children. In P. E. Spencer, C. J. Erting, & M. Marschark (Eds.), The deaf child in the family and at school (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.. Munson, L. J., & Odom, S. L. (1995). Infant-Caregiver Interaction Scale. Unpublished Manuscript, Portland State University Average Ratings I-CommI-TouchC-CommC-Touch Infant-Caregiver Communication & Touch Deaf-deaf Behaviors - All Ages Figure 1 D-d 9 D-d 12 D-d Average Ratings I-CommI-TouchC-CommC-Touch Infant-Caregiver Communication & Touch Hearing-deaf Behaviors - All Ages Figure 2 H-d 9 H-d 12 H-d 18