Rescorla's Correlation *Experiments * Note that Rescorla referred to his experiments as contingency experiments, however since a true contingency (cause-effect.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Classical Conditioning II
Advertisements

Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning
Probability Unit 3.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning.
COURSE: JUST 3900 INTRODUCTORY STATISTICS FOR CRIMINAL JUSTICE Instructor: Dr. John J. Kerbs, Associate Professor Joint Ph.D. in Social Work and Sociology.
LIAL HORNSBY SCHNEIDER
Psychology 485 September 28,  Introduction & History  Three major questions: What is learned? Why learn through classical conditioning? How does.
Inhibitory Pavlovian Conditioning Stimuli can become conditioned to signal the absence of a US— such learning is called Inhibitory Conditioning CS+ = excitatory.
1 Matched Samples The paired t test. 2 Sometimes in a statistical setting we will have information about the same person at different points in time.
Lectures 7&8: Pavlovian Conditioning (Determining Conditions) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
Analyzing Contingencies in Classical Conditioning A session of classical conditioning consists of a series of trials separated by intertrial intervals.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 5 – The Role of Conditioning in Behavior.
8 TECHNIQUES OF INTEGRATION. In defining a definite integral, we dealt with a function f defined on a finite interval [a, b] and we assumed that f does.
Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Presented by: Heather Hays.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning.
Contingency Theory of Classical Conditioning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning.
Learning What is Learning? –Relatively permanent change in behavior that results from experience (behaviorist tradition) –Can there be learning that does.
Chapter 9 - Lecture 2 Computing the analysis of variance for simple experiments (single factor, unrelated groups experiments).
PSY402 Theories of Learning Wednesday January 15, 2003.
1. a < b means a is less than b
Relationships Among Variables
Lecture 3-2 Summarizing Relationships among variables ©
Principles of Behavior Change Classical Conditioning.
CHAPTER 2 LIMITS AND DERIVATIVES. 2.2 The Limit of a Function LIMITS AND DERIVATIVES In this section, we will learn: About limits in general and about.
Theoretical Analysis of Classical Conditioning Thomas G. Bowers, Ph.D. Penn State Harrisburg.
Learning Prof. Tom Alloway. Definition of Learning l Change in behavior l Due to experience relevant to what is being learned l Relatively durable n Conditioning.
An Information Processing Perspective on Conditioning C. R. Gallistel Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science.
4 th Edition Copyright 2004 Prentice Hall5-1 Learning Chapter 5.
Finding Limits Graphically and Numerically An Introduction to Limits Limits that Fail to Exist A Formal Definition of a Limit.
Conditioned Inhibition CS B CS C clicks Conditioned inhibition is an internal state that prevents an organism from making some response, like salivation.
CHAPTER 4 Pavlovian Conditioning: Causal Factors.
Chapter 6 Learning.
Psychology 2250 Last Class Characteristics of Habituation and Sensitization -time course -stimulus-specificity -effects of strong extraneous stimuli (dishabituation)
Given the marginal cost, find the original cost equation. C ' ( x ) = 9 x 2 – 10 x + 7 ; fixed cost is $ 20. In algebra, we were told that what ever was.
Classical Conditioning Underlying Processes and Practical Application.
Experimental Evidence  Rats drink little saccharin water at first but increase over time.  Loud tones (110 db) produce different responses depending.
SKINNER’S “THEORY” OF INSTRUMENTAL CONDITIONING
College Algebra Sixth Edition James Stewart Lothar Redlin Saleem Watson.
Factors Influencing Conditioning  CS and US Intensity, and Attention to the CS  Temporal relationship  Predictiveness  Preparedness  Redundancy 1.
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved Sec Absolute Value Equations and Inequalities.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. 13 Linear Correlation and Regression Analysis.
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved Sec
This screen shows two lines which have exactly one point in common. The common point when substituted into the equation of each line makes that equation.
Blocking The phenomenon of blocking tells us that what happens to one CS depends not only on its relationship to the US but also on the strength of other.
–1 –5–4–3–2– Describe the continuity of the graph. Warm UP:
PSY402 Theories of Learning Friday January 17, 2003.
2.6 Limits at Infinity: Horizontal Asymptotes LIMITS AND DERIVATIVES In this section, we: Let x become arbitrarily large (positive or negative) and see.
Learning & Memory JEOPARDY. The Field CC Basics Important Variables Theories Grab Bag $100 $200$200 $300 $500 $400 $300 $400 $300 $400 $500 $400.
Chapter 6: Descriptive Statistics. Learning Objectives Describe statistical measures used in descriptive statistics Compute measures of central tendency.
Copyright © 2013, 2009, 2005 Pearson Education, Inc. 1 3 Polynomial and Rational Functions Copyright © 2013, 2009, 2005 Pearson Education, Inc.
LIMITS The Limit of a Function LIMITS In this section, we will learn: About limits in general and about numerical and graphical methods for computing.
ATTRIBUTES OF LEARNING AND CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.
Rescorla-Wagner Model  US-processing model  Can account for some Pavlovian Conditioning phenomena: acquisition blocking unblocking with an upshift conditioned.
Graph Sketching: Asymptotes and Rational Functions
Part II: Two - Variable Statistics
The Rescorla-Wagner Model
Equations with Variables on Both Sides
Classical Conditioning, Continued
Linear Inequalities and Absolute Value
Attributes of Learning and Classical Conditioning
Classical Conditioning and prediction
Contingency Theory of Classical Conditioning
Pavlovian Conditioning: Mechanisms and Theories
a + 2 = 6 What does this represent? 2 a
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Rethinking Extinction
Presentation transcript:

Rescorla's Correlation *Experiments * Note that Rescorla referred to his experiments as contingency experiments, however since a true contingency (cause-effect relationship) does not exist between the CS & UCS in classical conditioning experiments, they are more properly described as correlation experiments.

CS-UCS relations (correlation) l Contiguity is necessary but NOT sufficient for classical conditioning to occur l There must also be a consistent relationship or correlation between the CS and the UCS. l To experience a reliable correlation between the CS and the UCS the subjects must be exposed to numerous instances of the CS and UCS, thus many trials are typically necessary for conditioning.

Types of correlations between the CS and the UCS - #1 l If the CS is a reliable predictor of the presence of the UCS, then the CS and UCS are positively correlated. CS UCS

Types of correlations between the CS and the UCS - #2 l If the CS is an unreliable predictor of the UCS, then the CS and UCS are not correlated. CS UCS

Types of correlations between the CS and the UCS -#3 l If the CS reliably predicts the absence of the UCS, then the CS and UCS are negatively correlated. CS UCS

Rescorla’s Equations l It is inconvenient to draw time lines for experiments with large number of trials. l We will use equations which describe the type of correlation that a subject experiences in a classical conditioning experiment.

Rescorla’s Equation describing a positive correlation between the CS & UCS l the probability (p) l of a UCS l given that (/) l a CS is present l *** is GREATER THAN*** l the probability (p) l of a UCS l given that (/) l NO CS is present p (UCS / CS) > p (UCS / No CS)

Rescorla’s Equation describing a positive correlation between the CS & UCS p (UCS / CS) > p (UCS / No CS) The left side of the equation simply notes the percentage of CSs that are temporally contiguous (paired) with a UCS. If p = 1.0 then 100% of CSs are paired with UCSs If p = 0.5 then 50% CSs are paired with UCSs and 50% of CSs are presented alone. If p = 0.0 then all the CSs are presented alone, there are no CS-UCS pairings.

Rescorla’s Equation describing a positive correlation between the CS & UCS p (UCS / CS) > p (UCS / No CS) The right side of the equation simply notes the percentage of time intervals without a CS in which a UCS occurs. If p = 1.0 then UCSs are presented on 100% of the time intervals with No CS present. If p = 0.5 then UCSs are presented on 50% of the time intervals with No CS present. If p = 0.0 then UCSs are never presented when No CS is present.

Rescorla’s Equation describing positive correlations between the CS & UCS p (UCS / CS)p (UCS / No CS) When these correlations are used in classical conditioning experiments the subjects show evidence of excitatory conditioning Notice that the percentage of contiguous CS-UCS pairings decrease from the top example to the bottom example

Rescorla’s Equation describing no correlation between the CS & UCS p (UCS / CS) = p (UCS / No CS) l the probability (p) l of a UCS l given that (/) l a CS is present l *** is EQUAL to *** l the probability (p) l of a UCS l given that l NO CS is present

Rescorla’s Equation describing no correlation between the CS & UCS p (UCS / CS)p (UCS / No CS) When these correlations are used in classical conditioning experiments the subjects show no evidence of conditioning

Rescorla’s Equation describing negative correlations between the CS & UCS p (UCS / CS) < p (UCS / No CS) p (UCS / CS)p (UCS / No CS) When these correlations are used in classical conditioning experiments the subjects show evidence of inhibitory conditioning

Calculate Rescorla’s equation using the time lines below CS UCS First take notice that time line is divided into 12 equal intervals of time. Next we will calculate the left side of the equation p (UCS / CS) ? p (UCS / No CS) There are 4 time intervals with a CS / 4 A UCS occurs in all 4 CS intervals 4 = 1.0 Therefore the probability of a UCS given the presence of a CS is 1.0

Calculate Rescorla’s equation using the time lines below CS UCS Next we will calculate the right side of the equation p (UCS / CS) ? p (UCS / No CS) There are 8 time intervals with No CS / 4 A UCS occurs in 0 of these No-CS intervals 4 = 1.0 Therefore the probability of a UCS given the absence of a CS is 0 = 0.0 / 80 >

Calculate Rescorla’s equation using the time lines below First take notice that time line is divided into 12 equal intervals of time. Next we will calculate the left side of the equation p (UCS / CS) ? p (UCS / No CS) There are 4 time intervals with a CS / 4 A UCS occurs in only 1 of the CS intervals 1 = 0.25 Therefore the probability of a UCS given the presence of a CS is 0.25 CS UCS

Calculate Rescorla’s equation using the time lines below Next we will calculate the right side of the equation p (UCS / CS) ? p (UCS / No CS) There are 8 time intervals with No CS / 4 A UCS occurs in 2 of these No-CS intervals 1 = 0.25 Therefore the probability of a UCS given the absence of a CS is 0.25 = 0.25 / 82 = CS UCS

Calculate Rescorla’s equation using the time lines below First take notice that time line is divided into 12 equal intervals of time. Next we will calculate the left side of the equation p (UCS / CS) ? p (UCS / No CS) There are 4 time intervals with a CS / 4 A UCS occurs in none of the CS intervals 0 = 0.0 Therefore the probability of a UCS given the presence of a CS is 0.0 CS UCS

Calculate Rescorla’s equation using the time lines below Next we will calculate the right side of the equation p (UCS / CS) ? p (UCS / No CS) There are 8 time intervals with No CS / 4 A UCS occurs in 3 of these No-CS intervals 0 = 0.0 Therefore the probability of a UCS given the absence of a CS is 0.38 = 0.38 / 83 < CS UCS

Summary l When subjects experience CSs and UCSs that are positively correlated they acquire a conditioned response to the CS; this is called excitatory conditioning. l When subjects experience CSs and UCSs that are negatively correlated responses are inhibited (not performed) when the CS is present; this is called inhibitory conditioning or conditioned inhibition.

Summary continued l When subjects experience CSs and UCSs that are NOT correlated they show no evidence of conditioning.

Vocabulary l positive correlation l negative correlation l excitatory conditioning l inhibitory conditioning or conditioned inhibition

Using Rescorla’s equations to show differences in conditioning despite fixed contiguity between groups p (UCS / CS)p (UCS / No CS) CS - Alone Trials after Acquisition

Contiguity as necessary but not sufficient l The results of the previous experiment, as well as the results of the blocking studies and other experiments, suggest that although contiguity is necessary for classical conditioning to occur it is not enough (not sufficient), the CS and the UCS must be correlated either positively or negatively.

Explanations of Rescorla’s Correlation Experiments l COGNITIVE BEHAVIORIST: a correlation is also necessary because the CS must be predictive or informative. 1. When a CS is positively correlated with a UCS the subjects learn that the CS predicts the presence of a UCS. 2. When a CS is negatively correlated with a UCS the subjects learn that the CS predicts the absence of a UCS. This is a highly cognitive explanation of classical conditioning.

Explanations of Rescorla’s Correlation Experiments l RADICAL BEHAVIORIST: positive and negative correlations affect the acquisition of conditioned responding because several basic, mechanistic principles are at work. For example, when several UCSs are presented alone to degrade the CS-UCS correlation the context becomes excitatory and blocks conditioning to the CS.