TEMPLATE DESIGN © 2008 A Production Study on Phonologization of /u/-fronting in Alveolar Context KATAOKA, Reiko

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Tom Lentz (slides Ivana Brasileiro)
Advertisements

Sounds that “move” Diphthongs, glides and liquids.
SPPA 403 Speech Science1 Unit 3 outline The Vocal Tract (VT) Source-Filter Theory of Speech Production Capturing Speech Dynamics The Vowels The Diphthongs.
Basic Spectrogram & Clinical Application: Consonants
Speech Perception Dynamics of Speech
Human Speech Recognition Julia Hirschberg CS4706 (thanks to John-Paul Hosum for some slides)
Effects of Competence, Exposure, and Linguistic Backgrounds on Accurate Production of English Pure Vowels by Native Japanese and Mandarin Speakers Malcolm.
CS 551/651: Structure of Spoken Language Lecture 12: Tests of Human Speech Perception John-Paul Hosom Fall 2008.
Darkness in /l/ as a gradual phonetic property. Evidence from three Catalan dialects Daniel Recasens Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona & Institut d’Estudis.
Coarticulation Analysis of Dysarthric Speech Xiaochuan Niu, advised by Jan van Santen.
The sound patterns of language
ENG 528: Language Change Research Seminar Sociophonetics: An Introduction Chapter 5: Vowels (Continued) Lindblom (1963), Undershoot.
Phonetic variability of the Greek rhotic sound Mary Baltazani University of Ioannina, Greece  Rhotics exhibit considerable phonetic variety cross-linguistically.
Nasal Stops.
The Sound Patterns of Language: Phonology
Hillenbrand: Vowels1 The Acoustics and Perception of American English Vowels.
Speech Science XII Speech Perception (acoustic cues) Version
Splice: From vowel offset to vowel onset FIG 3. Example of stimulus spliced from the repetitive syllables. EXPERIMENT 2 (Voicing ID) METHOD Speech materials:
Spoken Language Analysis Dept. of General & Comparative Linguistics Christian-Albrechts-Universität zu Kiel Oliver Niebuhr 1 At the Segment-Prosody.
Sentence Durations and Accentedness Judgments ABSTRACT Talkers in a second language can frequently be identified as speaking with a foreign accent. It.
PHONETICS AND PHONOLOGY
General Problems  Foreign language speakers of a target language cause a great difficulty to native speakers because the sounds they produce seems very.
Voice Onset Time as a Parameter for Identification of Bilinguals Claire Gurski University of Western Ontario London, ON Canada.
Experiment 2: MEG Study Materials and Methods: 11 right-handed subjects with 20:20 vision were run. 3 subjects’ data was discarded because of poor performance.
Development of coarticulatory patterns in spontaneous speech Melinda Fricke Keith Johnson University of California, Berkeley.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Perceptual compensation for /u/-fronting in American English KATAOKA, Reiko Department.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Listener’s variation in phoneme category boundary as a source of sound change: a case of /u/-fronting.
SPEECH PERCEPTION The Speech Stimulus Perceiving Phonemes Top-Down Processing Is Speech Special?
English Phonetics and Phonology Lesson 4A
Chapter three Phonology
GABRIELLA RUIZ LING 620 OHIO UNIVERSITY Cross-language perceptual assimilation of French and German front rounded vowels by novice American listeners and.
Consonants and vowel January Review where we’ve been We’ve listened to the sounds of “our” English, and assigned a set of symbols to them. We.
Phonetics HSSP Week 5.
Coarticulation in apraxia of speech: an acoustic study of non-words Authors : Sandra P Whiteside and Rosemary A. Varley.
Present Experiment Introduction Coarticulatory Timing and Lexical Effects on Vowel Nasalization in English: an Aerodynamic Study Jason Bishop University.
Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Bosch, L. (2009) Developmental shift in the discrimination of vowel contrasts in bilingual infants: is the distributional account.
Segmental factors in language proficiency: Velarization degree as a signature of pronunciation talent Henrike Baumotte and Grzegorz Dogil {henrike.baumotte,
Speech Perception 4/6/00 Acoustic-Perceptual Invariance in Speech Perceptual Constancy or Perceptual Invariance: –Perpetual constancy is necessary, however,
Nasal endings of Taiwan Mandarin: Production, perception, and linguistic change Student : Shu-Ping Huang ID No. : NA3C0004 Professor : Dr. Chung Chienjer.
1 Speech Perception 3/30/00. 2 Speech Perception How do we perceive speech? –Multifaceted process –Not fully understood –Models & theories attempt to.
CSD 5100 Introduction to Research Methods in CSD Observation and Data Collection in CSD Research Strategies Measurement Issues.
Adaptive Design of Speech Sound Systems Randy Diehl In collaboration with Bjőrn Lindblom, Carl Creeger, Lori Holt, and Andrew Lotto.
Results 1.Boundary shift Japanese vs. English perceptions Korean vs. English perceptions 1.Category boundary was shifted toward boundaries in listeners’
Speech Science Fall 2009 Oct 28, Outline Acoustical characteristics of Nasal Speech Sounds Stop Consonants Fricatives Affricates.
Speech Science IX How is articulation organized? Version WS
LATERALIZATION OF PHONOLOGY 2 DAY 23 – OCT 21, 2013 Brain & Language LING NSCI Harry Howard Tulane University.
Assessment of Phonology
Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: a perceptual illusion? Emmanual Dupoux, et al (1999) By Carl O’Toole.
Sensation & Perception
Lecture 2 Phonology Sounds: Basic Principles. Definition Phonology is the component of linguistic knowledge concerned with rules, representations, and.
1 Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception Sandra Anacleto uOttawa.
2.3 Markedness Differential Hypothesis (MDH)
Bosch & Sebastián-Gallés Simultaneous Bilingualism and the Perception of a Language-Specific Vowel Contrast in the First Year of Life.
Speech Perception.
Acoustic Phonetics 3/14/00.
Stop/Plosives.
A STUDY ON PERCEPTUAL COMPENSATION FOR / /- FRONTING IN A MERICAN E NGLISH Reiko Kataoka February 14, 2009 BLS 35.
Against formal phonology (Port and Leary).  Generative phonology assumes:  Units (phones) are discrete (not continuous, not variable)  Phonetic space.
A PRODUCTION STUDY ON PHONOLOGIZATION OF /U/-FRONTING IN ALVEOLAR CONTEXT Reiko Kataoka 10 January 2009 LSA annual meeting.
Sentence Durations and Accentedness Judgments
An Introduction to : a closer look at analysing vowels
an Introduction to English
Elaine R. Hitchcocka, Ph.D., Laura L. Koenigb,c, Ph.D.
Speech Perception.
Jessica McKee Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences
The Nature of Learner Language (Chapter 2 Rod Ellis, 1997) Page 15
Giovanni M. Di Liberto, James A. O’Sullivan, Edmund C. Lalor 
Speech Perception (acoustic cues)
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
A Japanese trilogy: Segment duration, articulatory kinematics, and interarticulator programming Anders Löfqvist Haskins Laboratories New Haven, CT.
Presentation transcript:

TEMPLATE DESIGN © A Production Study on Phonologization of /u/-fronting in Alveolar Context KATAOKA, Reiko Department of Linguistics, University of California at Berkeley, 1203 Dwinelle Hall, Berkeley, California Introduction Acoustic Analysis A. F1 and F2 measurements from Reference vowels Figure 1. A sample waveform and spectrogram of “That’s a who’d again,” highlighting the vowel [u]. 1)For all reference vowels, F1 and F2 were measured at the temporal midpoint (red line) of the voiced portion of the vowel (Figure 1). 2) Then median F1 and F2 values were calculated for the six tokens for each vowel category ([i], [ ɪ ], [ ɛ ], [æ], [ ɑ ], [ ʌ ], [ ʊ ], [u]) for each speaker. B.F1 and F2 measurements from Test and Control vowels Figure 2. A sample waveform and spectrogram of “That’s a who’d again,” highlighting the vowel [u]. 1)For all Test and Control vowels, F1 and F2 were measured at the point of lowest F2 (red line) in the interval between the release of a closure and the end of the vowel (Figure 2). 2)Then median F1 and F2 values were calculated for six tokens for each word category (each Test words, Control /bvd/ word, Control /hud/ word) for each speech rate for each speaker. C. Vowel normalization (Nearey 1978) Figure 3. A sample F1-F2 plots for Reference vowels, a ‘center’ (brown star) defined as a mean_F1, mean_F2 c oordinate on a logarithmic scale (ln), and a normalized F1 (NF1) and F2 (NF2) values, defined as differences from mean ln(F1) and mean ln(F2), respectively, for the vowel [i] of speaker #1. Following Nearey (1978), all F1 and F2 values were log-normalized as differences from mean-log F1 and mean-log F2, respectively. Mean-log F1/F2 were calculated separately for each speaker from the eight Reference vowels produced by that speaker (Figure 3). Materials and Methods Results A. Realization of /u/ in /DuD/, /bud/, and /hud/ (and Reference vowels) 1)Phonetic realization of /u/ are similar in /bud/ and /hud/. 2)Realization of /u/ in /DuD/ are distinctly fronted (blue oval) (Figure 4). Figure 4. Scatter plots for NF1 and NF2 for medium speech rate tokens. For Test words, each data point shows mean of five test words (‘dude’, ‘zoos’, ‘noon’, ‘toot’, ‘Seus’). (n=31) B.Segment duration at Fast, Medium, and Slow speech Speakers systematically varied speech rate, resulting in systematically greater segment durations as speech rate decreases (Figure 5). Figure 5. A bar graph for Mean segment durations for each speaker for each speech rate. The mean values were obtained from all tokens spoken in the specified speech rate. C.Effect of Speech Rates on Vowel Formants There is no discernible effect on NF1 in all three contexts. NF2 seems to become higher (lower in decimal scale in Hz) in /DuD/ but not in /bud/ or /hud/ (Figure 6). Figure 6. Scatter plots for NF1 and NF2 of /u/ in /DuD/, /bud/ and /hud/ in Fast, Medium, and Slow speech tokens. In the plots for /DuD/, each data point shows mean of five test words (‘dude’, ‘zoos’, ‘noon’, ‘toot’, ‘Seus’). (n=31) Results (cont.) Discussion Bibliography D.NF2 as a function of segment duration 1)NF2 of /u/ in /bud/ remain the same across different segment duration. 2)NF2 of /u/ in /hud/ and /DuD/ increases (decreases in decimal scale in Hz) as segment duration increases. 3)NF2 of /u/ in /DuD/ have markedly different distribution, and the regression line for this data group does not cross the regression line for /bud/ data group. 4)Regression lines for the /hvd/ and /DuD/ data are nearly parallel (Figure 7). Figure 7. Scatter plots of NF2 as a function of segment duration, showing non-convergence of NF2 of /u/ in /DuD/ with that in /bud/ and /hud/. 1)The results from the production data obtained from 31 speakers indicate that the high back vowels had considerably higher F2 values following alveolar consonants than bilabial or no consonants (the phonetic realization of /h/ is a voiceless copy of the following vowel). The difference does not go away when segment duration increases (up to 300+ ms). This suggests that speakers have distinct production goals for /u/s in alveolar contexts; that is, /u/-fronting in this context has been phonologized in American English. 2)The near parallel regression lines for the /DuD/ and /hud/ data further reinforce the idea of ‘separate articulatory goals’ for vowels in different contexts. 3)For /u/ in /bud/, NF2 values remains the same across speech rates. This seems to reflect the fact that the onset /b/ and the vowel /u/ share the same articulation and thus the lowest F2 is not affected by the degree of overlapping of adjacent articulations. 4)The results of this study seem to suggest that language users have many more distinctive sound categories than traditionally assumed. Further, coarticulation may be better understood as categorical phenomena than gradient phenomena. Even if it starts out as gradient effect, by using the same articulatory sequence again and again, language learners might inevitably develop stable categorical sequencial articulatory goals. Lindblom (1963): Vowel reduction in Swedish CVC sequences Found: Reduced ‘undershoot’ as segment duration increases  ‘Undershoot’ is due to automatic coarticulation (i.e., phonetic).  Invariant vowel target articulation Solé (1992): Vowel nasalization in American English and Spanish Found: Constant duration for nasalization in Spanish vs. variable duration as a function of segmental duration in English  In Spanish nasalization is purely phonetic  In American English nasalization is phonologized Phonological features resist durational manipulation. Phonetic coarticulation disappears if there is enough time for the articulator to reach its target position. Background: Previous Studies on Coarticulatory Variations Recently many researchers model sound change as the result of phonologization (Hyman 1976), a process whereby coarticulatory perturbation of a speech sound becomes intended properties of the sound. Although phonologization is a useful concept in that it emphasizes the cognitive role in sound change, that a contexually predictable feature becomes dissociate from its contexts (Ohala, 1981), diagnostic criteria for determining whether phonologization has or has not occurred when the context itself is still present have yet to be proposed. This paper demonstrates how production data of various speech rates may be used to differentiate phonologized speech variations from mechanically caused variations. The case in point is /u/-fronting in alveolar contexts in American English, e.g., ‘dude’ (/dud/) being pronounced as [d ʉ d]. On the one hand, the alveolar environment would be expected to raise the 2 nd formant of the vowel, thus leading to the vowel being fronted but is the extent of this fronting purely phonetic or could it have become phonological? Following Lindblom (1963) and Solé (1992), the question of whether /u/-fronting is phonetic or phonological was investigated by production experiment, where speakers were asked to produce various words and non-words of a /C1VC2/ form in carrier sentences, where the place of C2 was fixed to alveolar and the place of C1 varied among alveolar (e.g. /dud/), bilabial (e.g., /bud/) and ‘no place’ (i.e., /hud/), in slow, medium, and fast speech rate. Data collection: UC Berkeley, Phonology Lab Participants: Native speakers of American English 31 talkers (5 M, 10F; yrs old) Carrier Sentence: “That’s a _____ again.” Production List: (each word was repeated 6 times) Reference [hvd] (medium rate) Test [dvd] (fast, slow, medium rate) he’d[i]dude[dud] hid[ ɪ ]zoos [zuz] head[ ɛ ]noon [nun] had[æ]toot [tut] hot[ ɑ ]Seus [sus] (5*3*6=90 tokens) HUD[ ʌ ] hood[ ʊ ] Control [cvc] (fast, slow, medium rate) who’d[u] (8*6=48 tokens)booed[bud] who’d[hud] (2*3*6=36 tokens) Total: 174 tokens/speaker elicited m_ln(F1) = 6.4 m_ln(F2) = 7.6 (ln) vowel [i] [u] [ ɪ ] [ ʊ ] [ ɛ ] [ ʌ ] [æ] [ ɑ ] ln(F1) = 5.89 ln(F2) = 8.02 NF1 = 0.51 NF2 = Context /hid//hud/ /h ɪ d//h ʊ d/ /h ɛ d//h ʌ d/ /hæd//bud/ /h ɑ t//DuD/ Type Contr ol: /bud/ Ref: /hVd/ Test: /DuD/ Mean duration (pooled) Fast: ms Medium: ms Slow: ms Hyman, L. ( 1976 ). “Phonologization,” In Linguistic studies presented to Joseph H. Greenberg, edited by A. Juilland (Saratoga), pp Lindblom, B., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. ( 1967 ). “On the role of formant transitions in vowel recognition,” J. Acoust Soc. Am. 42, Nearey, T. ( 1978 ). Phonetic Feature Systems for Vowels. Indiana. Indiana University Linguistics Club. Ohala, J. J. ( 1981 ). “The listener as a source of sound change,” in Papers from Parasession on Language and Behavior, edited by C. S. Masek, R. A. Hendrick, and M. F. Miller (Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago), pp Solé, M. J. ( 1992 ). “Phonetic and phonological processes: the case of nasalization,” Language and Speech 35, 29–43.