Figure 5: Initial (left panel) and terminal (right panel) distances between the noses of the dodger and robber rats are plotted separately for dodging.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The bead maze: Adapting the water maze task to the human manipulatory scale E.L. Loda, A.B. Thoennes, J.R. Köppen, S.S. Winter, D.A. Hamilton, D.G. Wallace.
Advertisements

192-IgG Saporin lesions of the medial septum or nucleus basalis magnocellularis disrupt exploratory trip organization. D.G. Wallace*, S.S. Winter, M.M.
Original Figures for "Molecular Classification of Cancer: Class Discovery and Class Prediction by Gene Expression Monitoring"
AMPHETAMINE AND HALOPERIDOL MAY ALTER TEMPORAL PROCESSING DURING A NATURALLY OCCURRING BEHAVIOR P.S.Wallace 1* ; E.F.Field 2 ; I.Q.Whishaw 2 ; D.G.Wallace.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective cholinergic lesions of the medial septum disrupt dead reckoning based navigation M.M. Martin*; M.D. Schultz; S.S.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Blindly Biased: Restricting Cue Access Impacts Spatial Orientation in a Human Analogue of the Water Maze B.P. Apger, D.M.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation disrupts exploratory trip organization D.G. Wallace*; S.K. Knapp; J.A. Silver;
Methods Results Conclusions Walking the plank: Role of the medial septum in distance estimation S.S. Winter; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology,
Humans were able to accurately use dead-reckoning to estimate distance and direction on a smaller scale. Participants modulated their peak speed on the.
A Human Analogue of Rat Food Hoarding Behavior: Searching Path Kinematics Influence the Accuracy of Dead Reckoning S.N. Choudhry; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace*
Conclusions Both groups set up a home base under novel-dark and light conditions and display a similar topographical organization of exploratory trips.
Unilateral lesions of the medial agranular cortex produce a persistent disruption in the organization of food protection behaviors S. J. WAGNER, M. J.
Methods Results Conclusions Mammillothalamic tract lesions disrupt self-movement cue processing in a food hoarding task. *S.S. Winter; J.L. McMillin, D.G.
Methods Results Conclusions Getting lost in the dark: Disruption of directional but not distance estimates following mammillothalamic tract lesions *S.S.
Navigating with fingers and feet: Comparative analysis of human and rat movement kinematics during non-visual spatial tasks J.R. Raines, N.D. McNeal, J.L.
Figure 4: Average dodger speeds are shown for MS (left panel) and NB (right panel) groups. Dodging (circles) and bracing (triangles) speeds are plotted.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Running on rungs: Serial pattern learning on the horizontal ladder *M.J Hylin 1 ; S.S. Winter 1 ; T. L. Marsiglio 1 ;
Participants: 21 smokers (13M, ages 18-45) and 21 age-, gender-, race-, and education-matched controls. Procedure: Stimuli were 100 photographs: 50 food.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Does the home base anchor spatial mapping? *D.G. Wallace 1 ; D.A. Hamilton 2 ; A.D. Choate 1 1 Department of Psychology,
Do Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Ever Return Home? S.L.Bennett 1* ; D.A.Hamilton 2 ; D.G.Wallace 1 1. Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb,
Figure 12: Each group’s average correlations are plotted for cued, uncued and dark probes. Group differences were observed during the uncued and dark probes.
Lesions of the Basal Forebrain Cholinergic System Impair Task Acquisition and Abolish Cortical Plasticity Associated with Motor Skill Learning James M.
Neglecting to protect: DCS lesions disrupt food protection behavior organization. 1 Blankenship, P.A., 1 Wolf, C. N., 2 Cheatwood, J.L., & 1 Wallace, D.G.
Making sense of the senseless: Evaluating sex differences of ASO
Figure 1. Whisker-evoked intrinsic signal in S1
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Cortico-Accumbens Regulation of Approach-Avoidance Behavior Is Modified by Experience and Chronic Pain  Neil Schwartz, Catriona Miller, Howard L. Fields 
Ranulfo Romo, Adrián Hernández, Antonio Zainos  Neuron 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Jung Hoon Sul, Hoseok Kim, Namjung Huh, Daeyeol Lee, Min Whan Jung 
Volume 48, Issue 5, Pages (December 2005)
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Volume 18, Issue 4, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Fear Conditioning in Humans
Inversely Active Striatal Projection Neurons and Interneurons Selectively Delimit Useful Behavioral Sequences  Nuné Martiros, Alexandra A. Burgess, Ann.
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity  Rodrigo M. Braga, Randy L. Buckner 
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Between Thoughts and Actions: Motivationally Salient Cues Invigorate Mental Action in the Human Brain  Avi Mendelsohn, Alex Pine, Daniela Schiller  Neuron 
Volume 38, Issue 5, Pages (June 2003)
Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration
Volume 92, Issue 2, Pages (October 2016)
Jonathan J. Nassi, David C. Lyon, Edward M. Callaway  Neuron 
Inhibitory Interplay between Orexin Neurons and Eating
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Gating of Fear in Prelimbic Cortex by Hippocampal and Amygdala Inputs
Volume 94, Issue 4, Pages e8 (May 2017)
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Peter Janssen, Rufin Vogels, Yan Liu, Guy A Orban  Neuron 
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Feng Han, Natalia Caporale, Yang Dan  Neuron 
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Volume 21, Issue 9, Pages (November 2017)
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Cortical Motion Deafness
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Orienting Attention Based on Long-Term Memory Experience
New and Distinct Hippocampal Place Codes Are Generated in a New Environment during Septal Inactivation  Mark P. Brandon, Julie Koenig, Jill K. Leutgeb,
Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum  John P. O'Doherty, Tony W.
Volume 39, Issue 5, Pages (August 2003)
Mobility of Synaptic Vesicles in Different Pools in Resting and Stimulated Frog Motor Nerve Terminals  Michael A. Gaffield, Silvio O. Rizzoli, William.
Acquisition of trace 50–250 is dependent on a subregion of mPFC in mice. Acquisition of trace 50–250 is dependent on a subregion of mPFC in mice. A, CR.
Volume 34, Issue 4, Pages (May 2002)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Volume 86, Issue 1, Pages (April 2015)
Presentation transcript:

Figure 5: Initial (left panel) and terminal (right panel) distances between the noses of the dodger and robber rats are plotted separately for dodging and bracing behaviors. The NB group initiated dodges at a farther distance from the robber than the other groups. No group differences were observed at the initiation of bracing or at the termination of dodging or bracing behaviors. Organization of food protection behavior is differentially influenced by hippocampal and cortical cholinergic deafferentation M.M. Martin*; L.A. Carter; J.L. Jones; S.S. Winter; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb, IL, USA Abstract Female Long Evans rats received either injections of 192 IgG-Saporin (SAP) or saline (control) into the medial septum (MS) or nucleus basalis (NB). Subsequent to recovery, rats were placed into an enclosure and provided a hazelnut in the presence of an unoperated conspecific. All rats engaged in dodging or bracing behaviors to prevent the theft of the hazelnut. Dodging involves the rat placing the food item in its mouth in order to use both fore- and hind limbs to escape the approaching conspecific. In contrast, bracing enables a rat’s forelimbs to maintain contact with the food item, and only the hind limbs are used to make shorter lateral movements. Several measures were analyzed to reflect attentional, motivational, and temporal influences on behavior. Figure 2: Behaviors were recorded from underneath, as shown in a photograph of the apparatus with a dodger and robber present (left column). Digitized examples are shown for a dodge (middle column) and brace (right column) by a dodger rat (blue) in an attempt to escape the robber rat (red). Dots represent digitization of the rats’ bodies: tip of nose, between the forelimbs, and base of tail. Black lines represent the starting locations of the dodger and robber rats (middle and right columns). Studies have demonstrated different roles for hippocampal and cortical cholinergic function in processing of temporal information in standard interval timing procedures. Previous work has suggested that rats use temporal information to organize their food protection behaviors as well. The present study examined the role of hippocampal and cortical cholinergic function on the organization of food protection behavior. Correspondence: M. Martin D. Web: Support Contributed By: NINDS grant NS Method Results Conclusions Figure 4: Distance (left panels) and average speed (right panels) are plotted separately for dodging and bracing behaviors. No group differences were observed in either measure. Figure 3: Time to consume a hazelnut (left column), total number of food protection behaviors (middle column) and percentage of successful behaviors (right column) are shown across groups. There was no group difference in time to consume a hazelnut or total number of food protection behaviors; however, the NB group was less successful in protecting food than control and MS groups. Figure 1: Photographs of coronal sections stained for AChE are presented from representative control, MS SAP, and NB SAP rats (left figure). The reduction in AChE was primarily restricted to the hippocampus in the MS SAP group and primarily restricted to the cortex in the NB SAP group (right figure) * Dodge Brace Figure 6: Temporal organization of food protection behaviors normalized by consumption time are plotted for representative control (top row), MS SAP (middle row), and NB SAP (bottom row) rats. Closed circles represent dodges, and open circles represent braces. Figure 7: Sum of time spent dodging and bracing are presented for control (left panel), MS SAP (middle panel), and NB SAP (right panel) groups for early and late samples. The control and NB SAP groups demonstrated an interaction between time spent dodging and bracing in early versus late samples of eating a single food item. The MS SAP group spent more time dodging than bracing and unlike the control and NB SAP groups, did not modulate time spent dodging and bracing. Control MS SAP NB SAP % Trial Time Groups did not differ in amount of time to eat a food item, number of food protection behaviors, distance traveled, or average speed, suggesting groups possessed similar motivation and motor abilities. The NB SAP rats were less successful in protecting their food and initiated dodges when the robber was farther away, relative to control and MS SAP groups. These impairments are consistent with an attentional deficit. The MS SAP group displayed a disruption in temporal organization of food protection behaviors, relative to control and NB SAP groups. This impairment is consistent with a deficit in temporal organization of food protection behavior. These results demonstrate a double dissociation between NB SAP and MS SAP involvement in organization of food protection behavior.