Tense as a clinical marker for SLI 37-975-01 Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
CHAPTER 2 THE NATURE OF LEARNER LANGUAGE
Advertisements

1 Reproduced by kind permission of Erik Smitterberg (PhD, Docent), Department of English, Uppsala University A-level Grammar 4: Verbs and Verb Phrases.
How Children Acquire Language
Developmental Sequences in Second Language Learning Presenters: Jacqueline dos Anjos, Hanna Heseker, Dana Meyer.
Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Grant Goodall Dept. of Linguistics.
NP Movement Passives, Raising: When NPs are not in their theta positions.
Passive comprehension by Slovak typically and atypically developing children Master thesis in psychology, Author: Radka Antalíková, Supervisor: Kristine.
Linguistic Theory Lecture 7 About Nothing. Nothing in grammar Language often contains irregular paradigms where one or more expected forms are absent.
Funding for this research is provided by the National Science Foundation, Grant Number SBE to the Pittsburgh Science of Learning Center (PSLC,
Sentence Repetition Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.
Language Acqisition - From Womb to School. Content Pre/Postnatal Language Development The First Three Years The Pre-School Years The School Years.
For more information, please write to: * This research was partially supported by the Israel Science Foundation (Grant No. 4806)
Subjectless Sentences in Child Language
1 The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: The Passive.
Non-Word Repetition Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.
Chapter 12: 2 nd Lang. Acq. As coffee is an ACQUIRED taste…
Week 2a. Morphosyntactic features, part II. Ch. 2, 4.2- CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
1 Indicators of SLI in bilingual children: inflections and prepositions Sharon Armon-Lotem & Joel Walters The Bilingual SLI Project Bar-Ilan University,
Verb inflections as indicators of Bilingual SLI Sharon Armon-Lotem, The Bilingual SLI project* Bar Ilan University *This project is funded by ISF grant.
Verb inflectional morphology in L2. Ludovica Serratrice (2001) The emergence of verbal morphology and the lead-lag pattern issue in bilingual acquisition”
Grammatical frameworks Inflectional morphology. Grammar In the Middle Ages, grammatica […] chiefly meant the knowledge or study of Latin, and were hence.
Psycholinguistics 11 Later language Acquisition. Acquisition of Morphology Order of Morpheme acquisition OrderMorpheme 1Present progressive 2-3Prepositions.
Corpus Linguistics Case study 2 Grammatical studies based on morphemes or words. G Kennedy (1998) An introduction to corpus linguistics, London: Longman,
Language Assessment of Bilingual Children. Information about bilinguals in U.S. Bilinguals not “two monolinguals in one” (Grosjean, 1989) Bilinguals use.
Emergence of Syntax. Introduction  One of the most important concerns of theoretical linguistics today represents the study of the acquisition of language.
Embedded Clauses in TAG
Thinking about inflections How to find verb inflections (Part of Dick Hudson's web tutorial on Word Grammar)web tutorial.
Learning the passive in natural(istic) settings Katie Alcock, Ken Rimba, Manizha Tellaie, and Charles Newton Thanks to Kamil ud Deen.
Paradigm based Morphological Analyzers Dr. Radhika Mamidi.
Dr. Monira Al-Mohizea MORPHOLOGY & SYNTAX WEEK 11.
Participants 81 children in second grade were divided into four sub-groups: Elicitation Task Comparing morphological errors across tasks in elementary.
Assessment of Morphology & Syntax Expression. Objectives What is MLU Stages of Syntactic Development Examples of Difficulties in Syntax Why preferring.
© Child language acquisition To what extent do children acquire language by actively working out its rules?
The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: From Tense to Movement Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment.
1 ASSESSING LANGUAGE KNOWLEDGE: GRAMMAR & VOCABULARY Prepared by Maria Verbitskaya, Elena Solovova, Svetlana Sannikova Based on material by Carolyn Westbrook.
A Longitudinal Study of Complex Syntax Production in Children with SLI There are relatively few studies of complex syntax (CS) in children with SLI (Schuele.
Verb phrases Main reference: Randolph Quirk and Sidney Greenbaum, A University Grammar of English, Longman: London, (3.23 – 3.55)
Unit 2 The Nature of Learner Language 1. Errors and errors analysis 2. Developmental patterns 3. Variability in learner language.
Basic Syntactic Structures of English CSCI-GA.2590 – Lecture 2B Ralph Grishman NYU.
SYNTACTIC DEVELOPMENT ECSE 500 CLASS SESSION 6. REVIEW PHONOLOGY SEMANTICS MORPHOLOGY TODAY - SYNTAX.
PRESCHOOLERS’ DEVELOPING MORPHOSYNTACTIC SKILLS. Power Point Outline** I. Mastering Grammatical Morphemes II. Developing Syntactic Elements III. Basic.
Language Development. Four Components of Language Phonology sounds Semantics meanings of words Grammar arrangements of words into sentences Pragmatics.
Alternative Approaches to the Role of Previously Known Languages Avoidance: when speaking or writing a second/foreign language, a speaker will often try.
Inflection. Inflection refers to word formation that does not change category and does not create new lexemes, but rather changes the form of lexemes.
X-Bar Theory. The part of the grammar regulating the structure of phrases has come to be known as X'-theory (X’-bar theory'). X-bar theory brings out.
A. Baker, J. de Jong, A. Orgassa & F. Weerman Collaborators: VARIFLEX project: Elma Blom & Daniela Polišenská (NWO-research grant : Disentangling.
INTRODUCTION TO THE HEBREW VERB and QAL PERFECT
1 Paradigmas Linguisticos Semester II Child language learning.
Week 3. Clauses and Trees English Syntax. Trees and constituency A sentence has a hierarchical structure Constituents can have constituents of their own.
REQUIREMENTS: A child must interact with other language users. A child must have the physical ability to send and receive information. P149.
Week 10 X-bar syntax: More on Clauses English Syntax.
1 U210B Chapter 1: ENGLISH AS A FIRST LANGUAGE Presentation: Dr. Faisal Al-Qahtani.
This use of single words as skeletal sentences is referred to as the holophrastic stage.
Child Syntax and Morphology
Non-finite forms of the verb
Week 10 X-bar syntax: More on Clauses
Verbal inflection: why is it vulnerable in SLI?
Lecture 3: Functional Phrases
Copyright © American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Copyright © American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Copyright © American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Verbs, tense, aspect, and mood
Telegraphic speech: two- and three-word utterances
Verbs.
Non - Finites A nonfinite verb is any of several verb forms that are not finite verbs; that is, they cannot serve as the root of an independent clause.
THE NATURE of LEARNER LANGUAGE
Root Infinitives in L2 – Supplement
The Nature of Learner Language
This use of single words as skeletal sentences is referred to as the holophrastic stage.
Roger Brown’s (1973) First Language Development Study and MLU
Presentation transcript:

Tense as a clinical marker for SLI Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University

The phenomenon Up to the age of three children use the infinitival form of the verbs in indicative matrix clauses in 50% of their verbal utterances in English (Wexler 1994), and to a lesser extent in other languages (Armon-Lotem 1996a, Hyams 1995, Rhee & Wexler 1995, Rizzi 1994a). Finite sentences are produced at the same time Children seem to know the grammatical properties of finiteness and non-finiteness (e.g., Deprez & Pierce 1994)

1) a. It only write on the pad b. He bite me c. My finger hurts 2) M: ma at osa? what you do 'what are you doing ? L:tapuax lishtot (Lior 1;08) apple to-drink 'I drink an apple'

Infinitival forms constitute only 5% of the Italian data. >>> Extensive use of root infinitives correlates with non-null subject languages. “A language goes through an OI stage if and only if the language is not an INFL-licensed null- subject language.” (Wexler 1996(

Some of the possible accounts Finite and non-finite forms are used randomly Wexler (1994), Harris & Wexler (1996): Children optionally omit tense. The use of root infinitives relates to a defective Tense that has not matured yet. Rizzi (1994a,b): the use of root infinitives relates to children’s lack of knowledge of the functions of C (e.g. C=ROOT). Truncated trees are not available once CP becomes obligatory both in matrix and embedded contexts Schutze & Wexler (1996): AGR/TNS omission model (ATOM) - children omit either TNS or AGR or neither Wexler (1998): Children assume the unique checking hypothesis – The features of a DP can only check against one functional category.

Wexler (1994) There is a correlation between the use of root infinitives and the use of bare negation. “A child who is optionally “dropping” 3rd [person] singular, will have medial negative sentences [of type I] in which the s does not appear on the verb“ (Wexler, 1994: 331): Type I: Mary not play baseball Type II: * Mary not plays baseball. >>> When negation precedes the verb, the verb should not be inflected.

Harris & Wexler (1996) Bare negation used at the optional infinitive-stage are largely (over 90%) of type I. This proportion is larger than the share of root infinitives. (Table 5) Do is omitted optionally where required for the same reason that -s is omitted optionally where required. Nevertheless, do-support is used more than Tense in general. (Table 11) Tensed forms are used almost completely correctly. (Tables 8 & 9) The bare stem is used both in present (64%) and non present (47%) contexts. (Table 12)

Schutze & Wexler (1996): AGR/TNS omission model (ATOM) Non-nominative case on subjects used at the optional infinitive-stage will be largely with non-finite verbs Only 5% of finite verbs take a non-nominative subjects, whereas 46% of non-finite verbs take a non-nominative subject. Non-nominative is the default case. (Test: “ Who wants ice- cream? ” )

a)AGR or TNS or both may be deleted b)AGR assigns NOM. If no AGR, subject gets default case c)Default case in English is ACC d) AGR checks 3rd person singular morphology Him goes is not attested because there is a contradiction between the verb morphology and the case on the subject.

Armon-Lotem (1996) for Hebrew There’s a gradual increase in the use of inflected verbs. Past tense morphology is acquired prior to person morphology, but this does not correlate with a decrease in the use of root infinitives, but rather with a decrease in the use of “stem-like forms”. The use of root infinitives reduces (from 5% to less than 1%) only when questions (and subordination) are mastered (last stage of Klima & Bellugi 1966).

Extended optional infinitive in English (Rice & Wexler 1995) Morphemes checked: 3rd person –s, past tense –ed, copula & auxiliary BE, and DO. Procedure: natural language samples + probe procedure aimed at elicitation (playing with toys). Subjects: SLI, N3 (language matched by MLU), N5 (age matched)

Findings - Percentage correct probes and spontaneous speech

Children in the SLI group showed a lower level of use in obligatory context than children in either control groups. N5 used the morphemes over 90% of the times, while SLI used them in 25%-48% of the time. N3 where in between (45%-70%) Error of use are very rare (SLI accuracy of agreement for probe data is like N3: 94% with BE, 82% with DO) Other morphemes, e.g., plural, are almost at normal level Prepositions are intact >> Tense marking is optional for a protracted period of time

Inflections in Hebrew speaking children with SLI Dromi, E. & S. Davidson A Clinical Marker for HSLI: from Empirical Findings to Theorizing. Paper presented at Brain and Language: Language Acquisition in Special Populations, Bar Ilan University, June. Dromi, E., Leonard, L., Adam, G. & Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S Verb Agreement Morphology in Hebrew-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, Dromi, E., Leonard, L.B., & Adam, G Evaluating the morphological abilities of Hebrew- speaking children with SLI. Amsterdam Series in Child language Development, 6, 65-78, Dromi, E., L. B. Leonard, and M. Shteiman (1993) The grammatical morphology of Hebrew-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment: some competing hypotheses. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36:

The morphological richness hypothesis SLI children have a limited processing capacity. They focus on the most salient aspects of the language they acquire. For example, in English they focus on word-order and ignore the morphology, while in German they focus on morphology and ignore the word order. Subjects: SLI, NDA, NDL (matched by MLU)

Dromi, E., L. B. Leonard, and M. Shteiman (1993) Findings: “Hebrew speaking children with SLI resembled their MLU controls in their use of both present and past tense inflections requiring agreement with the subject”. In the nominal system, plural formation, adjectival agreement, and the use of the accusative case marker are all delayed, but not different from language matched controls. >>> SLI is a delay

Dromi, E., Leonard, L., Adam, G. & Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S. (1999) Method: Sentence completion for 3rd person. Enactment tasks for 1st and 2nd person. 4 conjugations: pa'al, piel, hitpael, hif'il. The inflectional paradigm for past and present.

Findings In present tense, both SLI and NDL used past for preset In present tense, both SLI and NDL used masculine for feminine in singular and plural. SLI found Hitpa'el more difficult – using p'iel instead. Simplifying consonants cluster. SLI found Hif'il more difficult – using present for past and vice versa, using infinitives. SLI found pi'el more difficult – they used also stripped forms In past tense, 3rd person singular replaced many of the inflected forms. SLI used it mostly instead of other singular forms (56/64) – mostly for 2nd person NDL used it mostly instead of plural forms. Past tense does pose a problem for Hebrew speaking SLI children, whereas difficulties with present tense are less pronounced. Most errors were mostly related to the use of tense (60/144)) or person (67/144), but usually not both. Most errors were different by one feature from the target (77% in the past tense) >>> A limited processing capacity, since more complex structures, which place more demands on the system, seem to be more impaired.

Blass A Method: Spontaneous speech samples of the same children Findings: No difference between SLI and NDL in the level of inflections No difference between SLI and NDL in the mastery of inflections Out of all forms in Pa’al (80% of verbs), 90% were tensed. SLI used more bare (stripped) forms – significant, but the numbers are small. SLI and NDL had similar errors, but SLI had more. In natural settings children do what they know and avoid the difficult forms. >>>Delay

Davidson, S The Language Profile of Hebrew Speaking Preschoolers with Specific Language Impairement. M.A. Thesis, TAU. Methodology: H-IPSyn Findings: SLI are similar to NDL but for three criteria: Lexicon - SLI use a smaller variety of verb types than NDL Mrpho-syntax - SLI make more errors than NDL but of the same kind Pragmatic (??)- SLI have difficulties with reference not found in the NDL group

Passive Participle vs. Regular Past Tense Laurence B. Leonard, Patricia Deevy, Carol A. Miller, Leila Rauf, Monique Charest, and Robert Kurtz Surface Forms and Grammatical Functions: Past Tense and Passive Participle Use by Children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol The girl pushed the boy. The boy got pushed by the girl. EOI account: different The surface account: same

Method Subjects  12 of the children (aged from 4,6 to 6, 10) with SLI  12 ND-A  12 ND-MLU Sentence completion tasks: the use of past tense verb forms the use of passive participle verb forms

Summary The inconsistency with which children with SLI produce past – ed cannot be due to the surface property of this inflection. Its grammatical function probably plays the central role. Children with SLI have special problems with verb morphology, even when tense is not involved. The passive participle – ed proved to be one such area of weakness.