PSY205s: The psychology of aviation - Situational Awareness Dave Nunez, MPhil Department of Psychology University of Cape Town.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chia Wei Ensar Becic Christopher Edwards HumanFIRST Program Department of Engineering University of Minnesota.
Advertisements

In need of a model for complexity assessment of highly automated human machine systems Fredrik Barchéus, Pernilla Ulfvengren, Johan Rignér.
Royal Free Hampstead NHS Trust The Human Issues: -Why Exposures Happen -Responding to Reduce Distress Barbara Wren, C.Psychol. Occupational Health Psychologist.
The Driving Task The driving task is everything it takes to operate a motor vehicle. The three skills of the driving task are: A. Physical-coordination.
1 Cognition of spatial situations Moving through a populated space is a complex task –Eg. Walking in a crowd –Combines aspects of perception, memory, expectations,
Perceptual Processes: Attention & Consciousness Dr. Claudia J. Stanny EXP 4507 Memory & Cognition Spring 2009.
Chapter 3 Attention and Performance
The Importance of Synergy Between Flight Deck and Cabin Crews
1 Situation Awareness for CFIs Donna Forsyth Wilt, Ph.D. Florida Institute of Technology Presented at CFI Workshop FAA Safety Center, Lakeland, FL May.
COGNITIVE VIEWS OF LEARNING Information processing is a cognitive theory that examines the way knowledge enters and is stored in and retrieved from memory.
P-2028 Minnesota Wing Aircrew Training: Tasks P-2028 Crew Resource Management.
Effects of Automation in the Aircraft Cockpit Environment
Stress, Workload, Accidents, & Errors
Cognitive Load Theory Sweller, van Merrienboer, and Paas, 1998 Psych 605 Fall 2009.
1 Enhancing Flight Crew Management of Unexpected Events This presentation is intended to enhance the reader's understanding, but it shall not supersede.
1 ISE 412 Stress, Workload, Accidents, & Errors Figure 13.1 A representation of stress effects. (Wickens, pg.325) Information Processing Stressors Experience.
Design Activities in Usability Engineering laura leventhal and julie barnes.
HCI in Safety-Critical Applications: ATC Systems Aslan Neishaboori.
Flight Department 1 A Research and a Result Barcelona October 2003 Capt. Cor Blokzijl, Aero Lloyd 10 th Airbus Flight Safety Conference.
Using Situational awareness and decision making
Situational Awareness / Photo and Video Flights WHY DISCUSS? Scratch Mitchell – CJAA 2015.
Mission Aircrew School Chapter 14: Crew Resources Management (March 2011)
Stress and Workload Chapter 13. Overview of Stressors Psychological Threat Threat Anxiety Anxiety Fatigue Fatigue Frustration Frustration Anger Anger.
Human Supervisory Control Memory & Attention.
Attention & Human Decision Making  Decision Making is the end goal of Human Information Processing.  However, people are not optimal decision makers,
Chapter 7: Memory and Training Slide Template. WORKING MEMORY.
Human factors in Complex Aviation Systems Cognition and some related errors.
Attention as a Limited Capacity Resource
The Field Guide to Human Error Investigations- The Old View (Chapters 1 – 6) By Dekker AST 425.
Hard Work and Vigilance: Necessary but Insufficient The Role of Human Factors in General Practice Dr Richard Jenkins Tuesday 2 nd November 2010.
Downloaded from 9/14/2015 Aeronautical Decision Making - The Theory Behind the Practice 1 AERONAUTICAL DECISION MAKING The Theory Behind The.
Transportation Human Factors Chapter 17. Automotive Human Factors Importance – over 40,000 fatalities per year in the US with 90% attributable to human.
CREW RESOURCE MANAGEMENT
Comp 15 - Usability and Human Factors Unit 4a - Human Factors and Healthcare This material was developed by Columbia University, funded by the Department.
Synthetic Cognitive Agent Situational Awareness Components Sanford T. Freedman and Julie A. Adams Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science.
Situational Awareness Numerous aircraft and operational displays, when combined with effective and efficient communications and facilities, provide Air.
Npor. Ing. Bc. Monika Vaňková. Content ATC Physical factors Psychological factors Prevention.
1 IE 590D Applied Ergonomics Lecture 26 – Ergonomics in Manufacturing & Automation Vincent G. Duffy Associate Prof. School of IE and ABE Thursday April.
Copyright Catherine M. Burns
Usability and Human Factors Unit 4a Human Factors and Healthcare.
Attention Grounding: A New Approach to IVIS Implementation Emily Wiese Cognitive Systems Lab Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering University.
Comp 15 - Usability and Human Factors
Sensory and Working Memories Reviewing Behaviorism Information Processing Memory Test your perception—top down or bottom up.
Cognitive Theories of Learning Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos.
Chapter 13: Stress and workload
The Successful Aviation Professional AST 141. Professionalism Positive, courteous attitude Punctuality Appearance Good follow-through Concern for other.
Theories of Learning: Cognitive Theories Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos 15 May 2009.
Decision Making Chapter 7. Definition of Decision Making Characteristics of decision making: a. Selecting a choice from a number of options b. Some information.
February 15, 2004 Software Risk Management Copyright © , Dennis J. Frailey, All Rights Reserved Simple Steps for Effective Software Risk Management.
Human Abilities 2 How do people think? 1. Agenda Memory Cognitive Processes – Implications Recap 2.
U SER I NTERFACE L ABORATORY Situation Awareness Definitions and Terminology Situation Awareness, as a state of knowledge, from the processes (situation.
HUMAN FACTORS FOR INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE
1 ISE 412 ATTENTION!!! From page 147 of Wickens et al. ATTENTION RESOURCES.
U SER I NTERFACE L ABORATORY Situation Awareness a state of knowledge, from the processes used to achieve that state (situation assessment) not encompass.
Memory How do we retain information? How do we recall information?
Human Factors An Overview
Ergonomics/Human Integrated Systems (Project 02)
Lecture 11: Flight Management System (FMS)
Crew Resources Management Lecture 7: Workload Management.
Applying Human Factors Principles Chapter 10 Section B Aeronautical Decision Making.
Crew Resources Management
Michelle Hester Advanced Human Factors (PSYC 562)
Information Aids for Diagnosis Tasks Based on Operators’ Strategies 김 종 현.
Dr. Dennis Vincenzi Human Factors Issues Dr. Dennis Vincenzi
Strategic Team Decision Making Florida Reliability Coordinating
CRM 1204 CREW RESOURCE MANAGEMENT
Chapter 7: Memory and Training
Chapter 2 Human Information Processing
6. Cognition.
6. Cognition.
Presentation transcript:

PSY205s: The psychology of aviation - Situational Awareness Dave Nunez, MPhil Department of Psychology University of Cape Town

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 2 Admin info I am in room 4.22 (  ) Paper: Endsley 1999 from Work Return Room Slides for the time being: (on the course web page later)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 3 Why study aviation in psychology? Airplanes get more complex…. Bleriot Model XI Aerospatiale-BAC Concorde ….but the people who fly them remain the same.

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 4 Why study aviation in psychology? Goal: Increase safety (purely applied) Aviation is a potentially dangerous activity Increase safety by engineering (better engines, etc) What about the people doing the flying? How safe is flying? Deaths per year by cause (USA ) Commercial flight:100 Electrical current:850 Bicycle riding:1000 Pedestrian:8000 Falling:12000 Auto accidents:46000 So flying to Joburg should be about 460 times safer than driving there.

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 5 A little history During the ‘psychometrics boom’ (1920s- 1930s), psychologists get involved Pilot selection tests During WW2, psychologists begin to look at aviation loses More aircraft lost to accidents than the enemy! Bartlett, Chapanis, Craik, Gibson and other ‘big names’ got involved

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 6 Pilot error vs. Human error General rule was: If the plane didn’t fail, it was “pilot error” Pejorative phrase; laid blame Implication – ‘not good enough’ Evidence from Chapanis and others showed it was actually “human error” Acknowledge limits of human beings Certain system features create situations where an error is more likely Problem becomes worse under certain environmental conditions Alphonse Chapanis ( ), was a leading figure in the psychology of aviation safety since the 1940s

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 7 Example: Human error (and a solution) Problem: Pilots shut down engines in flight by pulling the wrong lever Chapanis’ solution: shape coding the lever handles (also color coded by function) Douglas DC-4 (early 1940s) Beechcraft Duchess (late 1970s)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 8 Human Factors (Ergonomics) Understand patterns of errors Many errors can occur regardless of experience What about being human makes us likely to commit errors? Examine cognitive processes to identify ‘danger spots’ Goal: To create systems which reduce the probability of errors ‘Pilot friendly’ aircraft which reduces errors and creates a better working environment

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 9 A major contribution: SA A recent important contribution: Situational awareness Combination of mental models, working memory and situated cognition theory Tries to predict how and when errors can happen Applied to operation of complex systems (nuclear powerplants, ships, aircraft, cars) Much research, and is taught to pilots Increase their awareness of when things can go wrong

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 10 A quick recap of the info-processing model [Herbert Simon’s model] Attention, WM (STM), LTM. Attention filters irrelevant (unexpected) stimuli out Stimuli are transformed in WM according to active rules and schema (from LTM) Contents of WM in turn activate rules and schema as required by the data Behaviour/consciousness is based on the contents of WM and active scripts

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 11 SA: definition “the perception of the elements in the environment within a volume of time and space, the comprehension of their meaning and the projection of their status in the near future” (Endsley 1998) Cognitive task (probably expertise bound) Dynamic (over time and space) Several levels of processing (perception, comprehension, prediction)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 12 Levels of SA (1) Level 1: Perception of the environment Other aircraft, terrain, own aircraft systems, navigation, radios Deals with the present – loads perceptual buffers, attention and working memory Mica Endsley (of SA Technologies, previously of MIT) is one of the leading experts on situational awareness in aviation

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 13 Levels of SA (2) Level 2: Comprehension Synthesis of disjoint level 1 elements (‘big picture’) Assign importance to each element (goal- directed) Forms a holistic understanding of what is happening now Reaching this level requires experience It is mostly a top-down task; loads working memory, and requires LTM

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 14 Levels of SA (3) Level 3: Projection (predicting) Requires both Level 1 and Level 2 SA Also expertise bound – More experienced pilots spend more time predicting what will occur Effectively gives the pilot more time for decision making The heavier an aircraft, the longer it takes to respond to a pilot’s input. In such situations, Level 3 SA is essential. This is partly why airlines pick their most experienced pilots to fly such aircraft.

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 15 Cognition and SA levels All three levels require some WM and attention Level 1: Speech comprehension, decoding system interfaces; attentional filtering (mostly bottom-up) Level 2: Activating mental models and schemata (mostly top-down) Level 3: Take LTM information and apply it to active models (mostly top-down) So WM and attention loads can be high during SA But: Experts will use less (better chunking strategies, better at filtering out irrelevant elements)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 16 Information load: Example Data Sources: Outside (terrain) Outside (weather) Outside (aircraft) Inside (gauges) Inside (maps) Inside (checklists) Inside (crew) Aural (crew) Aural (control) Aural (aircraft) Aural (alarms) Haptic (controls) Haptic (buffets, etc)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 17 Individual factors in SA (1) Limits of attention Novices or experts in novel situations require more attention to be placed on the environment Information overload can exceed the capacity – ‘miss’ important relevant information Giving more attention to one SA task reduces it on another Serious problem: NTSB review – 31% of human errors due to problems acquiring relevant data

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 18 Individual factors in SA (2) Can be overcome by ‘sampling’ information Learn a way to ‘scan’ the world (avoids fixation) Strongly trained patterns become habitual Sampling can fail: Non-optimal strategy (focus on the wrong things) Visual dominance (forget other inputs) Memory failures (forget relative importance of elements) In overload, ‘leave out’ certain elements

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 19 Individual factors in SA (3) Attention limits can be helped by expertise Top-down knowledge creates expectations, which can increase processing speed BUT: if unexpected information occurs, more likely to make an error (‘superiority’ effects) WM is used mostly for integration and projection (Levels 2 & 3) If much new info is being processed, little WM will be left for integration (and vice-versa!) Projection places a particularly heavy load on WM (need to store multiple states)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 20 Coping mechanisms (1) All is not lost – cognitive strategies/structures exist to deal with this ‘coping mechanisms’ (not really) Normal info sorting/learning structures Generally: use previous knowledge to order the world Some trained (automaticity); some developed (mental models) Generally automatic, subconcious processes

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 21 Coping mechanisms (2) Structured knowledge from experience (LTM) Schemata, scripts & mental models ‘fill in’ missing info (default values) Help with structuring & comprehension (reduce WM & attention used) Increase accuracy of predicting the future Can be a ‘fuzzy fit’ Almost essential for higher levels of SA

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 22 Coping mechanisms (3) Goal-driven processing Goals determine how resources are allocated also Goals provide a structure in which to process (allows higher levels of SA) Automaticity (habitual responses) – ‘scripted’ Allows processing with minimal attention Can miss novel stimuli Safe for routine situations (is there such a thing?)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 23 Putting it all together (Endsley 1995)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 24 Where the problems can occur

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 25 Factors which reduce SA – Stress (1) Physical stressors Noise, vibration, lighting, temperature, fatigue, ‘jet lag’ Social/psychological stressors Anxiety/fear, uncertainty, self- esteem, career advancement, time pressure Stress effects are complex – a little can help (yerkes- dodson law) Stress produces many physical and psychological effects which can reduce SA and undermine a pilot’s ability to act correctly.

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 26 Factors which reduce SA – Stress (2) Why does stress affect SA? Attentional narrowing (high arousal/anxiety) Oversampling of dominant cues Scan patterns disrupted Premature closure (hasty decisions) Reduction in WM capacity / LTM retrieval (affects Level 2 & 3 most) Training reduces these effects Automaticity reduces attention and WM requirements More cues, better associations improve retrieval

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 27 Factors which reduce SA – under/overload Mental overload WM and attention limits reached Incomplete/erroneous perception Stressor (being ‘behind the plane’) Mental underload No active search for info Low vigilance/motivation Air traffic controllers (ATCs) also require high levels of SA. In busy sectors (such as London, Atlanta or Tokyo) the volume of traffic can lead to mental overload.

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 28 Factors which reduce SA – bad systems The aircraft’s interface can present information poorly Presenting too much can lead to overload Hiding too much can lead to unawareness The layout of information can interfere with the scan Recently: ‘Smart’ planes (‘glass cockpit’) Aware of the information required in a flight phase Show what is necessary, but watch for problems in the background Alert the crew if a problem exists (speech, icons, etc)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 29 Improvements in interfaces Boeing Adv (late 1960s) Boeing (Early 2000s)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 30 Factors which reduce SA - Complexity Aircraft keep getting more complex Technology demands Increases workload – more system components, more interactions Effectively increases number of goals and tasks An expert in these systems will be protected (a little) Pilots vary widely on their self-reported understanding of the systems A difficult road to becoming an expert!

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 31 Factors which reduce SA – Automation (1) Habitual procedures can take crews ‘out of the loop’ Reduce vigilance, increase complacency Become a passive recipient of information Automatic states have bad cognitive consequences Pilots are slower to detect problems Slower to re-orient after realizing the problem (schemata de-activation/re-activation)

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 32 Factors which reduce SA – Automation (2) But: automation can help SA also Computers can monitor many variables Remove unnecessary manual work (navigation) Can present many variables already integrated (for Level 2) The trick is: create systems which aid but do not promote complacency

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 33 How serious is a failure in SA? Jones & Endsley looked at accidents in the USA over a 4 year period (major carriers) 77% had a substantial human error component Of those, 88% due to a failure of SA SA failures not even distributed among levels Level 1: 72% Level 2: 22% Level 3: 6%

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 34 Further: specific cause

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 35 Teamwork: SA in CRM Most aircraft are flown by a team Do other people increase or decrease SA? Spread the work: Effectively have more WM and attention But: Is that enough for collaboration? Can too many cooks spoil this broth?

PSY205s – The psychology of aviation: Situational Awareness 36 Sharing data To work together, people must share info Keep mental models etc. aligned Each must know what information the other needs Must also share higher level understanding, and projection (level 2 and 3) Essential: shared mental models High functioning crews communicate less than low functioning crews