Simple Rules and Metrical Phonology

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Syllable Structure in English
Advertisements

Phonology, part 7: Rule Types + Ordering
Phonology Organization and interaction of sounds in a language sound system.
The sound patterns of language
The Sound Patterns of Language: Phonology
1 Linguistics week 11 Finish assimilation; start morphology.
Linguistic Structures CSE 140/etc.. The central hypothesis Human intelligence is within the set of computable functions (Turing) Application to language.
Syllables and Stress, part II October 22, 2012 Potentialities There are homeworks to hand back! Production Exercise #2 is due at 5 pm today! First off:
Sound Structure Part II: Phonology
Phonology Phonology is essentially the description of the systems and patterns of speech sounds in a language. It is, in effect, based on a theory of.
Chapter 9 Allomorphy: Books with more than one cover Morphology Lane 333.
The sound patterns of language
Algebra Problems… Solutions Algebra Problems… Solutions © 2007 Herbert I. Gross Set 4 By Herb I. Gross and Richard A. Medeiros next.
Part Four PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSES.  Speech sounds are by nature dynamic and flexible, and highly susceptible to the influence of the ‘environment’, i.e.
Writing sounds changing
1 Analysis of Variance This technique is designed to test the null hypothesis that three or more group means are equal.
Six compound procedures and higher-order procedures.
Morphology I. Basic concepts and terms Derivational processes
Research on teaching and learning pronunciation
Chapter three Phonology
Meaning and Language Part 1.
Chapter7 Phonemic Analysis PHONOLOGY (Lane 335). What is Phonology? It’s a field of linguistics which studies the distribution of sounds in a language.
Lecture 1 Introduction: Linguistic Theory and Theories
ACE TESOL Diploma Program – London Language Institute OBJECTIVES You will understand: 1. A process for teaching the receptive and productive sides of pronunciation.
CSD 2230 HUMAN COMMUNICATION DISORDERS
MORPHOLOGICAL PROCESSES Dr. Monira I. AL-Mohizea.
Lecture for Week Spring.  Numbers can be represented in many ways. We are familiar with the decimal system since it is most widely used in everyday.
Positional Number Systems
…not the study of telephones!
Phonetics and Phonology
Ch 9 & Ch 10 Slide 1 Ch 9 – Productivity Productivity – the capacity of a rule to apply to novel circumstances. P. 190 Vowel nasalization in English is.
The Linguistics of Second Language Acquisition
1. Information Conveyed by Speech 2. How Speech Fits in with the Overall Structure of Language TWO TOPICS.
Main Topics  Abstract Analysis:  When Underlying Representations ≠ Surface Forms  Valid motivations/evidence or limits for Abstract Analysis  Empirical.
Phonology I Phonemes and Simple Rules. Connecting with last week Remember last week: the physics of speech. And, the idea that the consonant and vowel.
Phonology The sound patterns of language Nuha Alwadaani March, 2014.
Today we will talk about: -Phonology -Phonemes -morphemes.
The Great Vowel Shift Continued The reasons behind this shift are something of a mystery, and linguists have been unable to account for why it took place.
English Linguistics: An Introduction
Introduction to Linguistics Ms. Suha Jawabreh Lecture 9.
Ch 7 Slide 1  Rule ordering – when there are multiple rules in the data, we have to decide if these rules interact with each other and how to order those.
Ch 3 Slide 1 Is there a connection between phonemes and speakers’ perception of phonetic differences? (audibility of fine distinctions) Due to phonology,
Phonology, Part VI: Syllables and Phonotactics November 4, 2009.
Morphology A Closer Look at Words By: Shaswar Kamal Mahmud.
Levels of Language 6 Levels of Language. Levels of Language Aspect of language are often referred to as 'language levels'. To look carefully at language.
PROBLEM AREAS IN MATHEMATICS EDUCATION By C.K. Chamasese.
Big Ideas in Reading: Phonemic Awareness
Lecture 2 Phonology Sounds: Basic Principles. Definition Phonology is the component of linguistic knowledge concerned with rules, representations, and.
Rules, Movement, Ambiguity
Chapter II phonology II. Classification of English speech sounds Vowels and Consonants The basic difference between these two classes is that in the production.
[fon Є tiks and fon Ɔ logi] Weeks 2-4 [wiks tu to for] Phonetics and Phonology.
13 th October Vocabulary We use the correct terminology with the children right from Early Years. It may sound complicated but it actually makes.
Chapter 4: Phonology… …not the study of telephones! NOTES: The slides/lecture/discussion for this chapter deviate from the order of the book… You WILL.
THE SOUND PATTERNS OF LANGUAGE
3 Phonology: Speech Sounds as a System No language has all the speech sounds possible in human languages; each language contains a selection of the possible.
Ch4 – Features Features are partly acoustic partly articulatory aspects of sounds but they are used for phonology so sometimes they are created to distinguish.
Principles Rules or Constraints
LITERACY-BASED DISTRICT-WIDE PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Aiken County Public School District January 15, 2016 LEADERS IN LITERACY CONFERENCE.
Technische Universität München Introduction to English Pronunciation Syllable Structure.
Phonological Features And Natural Classes. So, remember features?  Remember how it used to be so simple?  Three little descriptors…  Place  Manner.
King Faisal University [ ] 1 E-learning and Distance Education Deanship Department of English Language College of Arts King Faisal University Introduction.
Revisiting common errors Tutor: Prof. Cecilia A. Zemborain Student: Ma. De los Ángeles Svidersky.
Introduction to Linguistics
عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد
Step 1: Memorize IPA - practice quiz today - real quiz on Tuesday (over consonants)! Phonology is about looking for patterns and arguing your assessment.
Phonology Indah Lestari.
Part I: Basics and Constituency
Introduction to Linguistics
Structural relations Carnie 2013, chapter 4 Kofi K. Saah.
Presentation transcript:

Simple Rules and Metrical Phonology Phonology II Simple Rules and Metrical Phonology

Plan In the last class, we saw how the phonology of a language operates in terms of features that collectively make up speech sounds It is essential to remember that phonological operations make reference to features; in the first part of today’s class, we’ll look at how to treat such operations in terms of rules. In the second part of the class, we’ll look at another level of phonological organization, which concerns how sounds are organized into syllables and larger units.

Review: Alternations In our last lecture, we saw alternations: cases in which an object that is “one thing” at some level of analysis surfaces with distinct variants That’s a more precise way of saying that e.g. the English plural morpheme has some underlying form (I.e., /z/), but surfaces in different forms: /z/, /s/, /@z/ This alternation is phonological in nature Other alternations are not; consider that the plural of box is boxes, but the plural of ox is oxen. This alternation is morphological nature.

Introducing phonological rules We need a way of formalizing these changes As a basic assumption, we’ll assume that every word and morpheme has an Underlying Representation, or UR. The UR is the basic sound form of that object; that is, how it is stored in the memory of speakers These UR’s are subject to operations-- rules-- that derive the surface forms of the words

Rules, abstractly In the basic form that we’ll be using here, rules have some sort of input that specifies what the rule applies to, and an output that shows the change that is effected Abstractly, these can be written as follows: X  Y/ __ Z Informally, a rule of this type says that X (the input) is converted into Y (the output) when X is to the left of Z.

An example Remember from above we saw some examples from Old English in which there’s a single phoneme underlying [f] and [v] on the surface Let’s assume that this is underlyingly /f/ Recall the distribution: [v] occurs between vowels. We can express this semi-formally as follows: f  v/ V__ V This rule says that f becomes v between vowels If we needed to be more precise about this, we could specify this in terms of features. Then, what would change is a specification of the [voc] feature (I.e., the rule would change it from [-voc] to [+voc] This more precise rule would have to be written to voice /f/ intervocalically! Notice that if we are not careful, we might have a rule that voices too many consonants

Another example Let’s take an assimilation process Consider (a subset of…) words prefixed with in- in English; these express contradiction/negation: inappropriate intolerant insincere In these forms, the idea is that we are adding a morpheme to express the negative part Based on the examples we see above, we would think that the nasal consonant in this morpheme is /n/

In-prefixation, continued Notice now that not all words with this prefix have the same pronunciation of that component: impossible imbalanced impure In fact, the change in pronunciation is even reflected in the spelling (not all changes are!) What’s the pattern? We get the m-realization in front of labial consonants (labial stops)

Accounting for the pattern There are some more details about the in- prefix that we could look at; for the moment, think just about the facts we have seen We want to say that a nasal consonant becomes labial when a labial stop follows. One way of doing this: C[+nas]  C[+nas,+lab]/__ C[+obst,+lab]

Think about it… The rule on the last slide shows how rules work. There’s actually more to be said about the facts of assimilation: Not all morphemes behave the same; compare unpopular with impossible… What about glides? I.e. unwelcome… are there any in- words like this? What about other consonants? How do people say e.g. incapable? What about unclear?

Types of rules: Summary Phonological rules effect many different types of changes: Some make adjacent sounds more similar; these are often called Assimilation rules (remember e.g. English plurals) There are also Dissimilation rules, that make nearby sounds more distinct Some rules add a consonant or a vowel or a consonant that isn’t there in an Underlying Representation. These are called rules of Epenthesis. The most important thing in most cases is to find the environment in which the rule is triggered, so that you are stating the generalization about the environment correctly

Rule Interactions When linguists analyze the phonology of a language, they often find that many different rules apply to derive the surface forms That is, any given word that you see might be the result of more than one rule One difficult issue is that in many cases, rules have to apply to a form in a specific order This is kind of an advanced topic, but worth considering

Illustration Here’s a simplified example; some nouns in Polish, in singular and plural: sg pl trans ruk rog’i ‘horn’ lut lodi ‘ice’ There are two things going on in these nouns: One rule devoices voiced consonants at the end of words Another rule raises /o/ to /u/ when a voiced consonant follows In order to get the correct results, the vowel raising rule has to apply before the consonant devoicing rule! Take home lessons: (1) there are some cases in which more than one rule has to be posited; (2) sometimes rule ordering is crucial

Syllables and Metrical Structure Transition: To this point, we have been looking at the smaller units in phonological analysis: features, and their arrangement into segments (along with the question of what the phoneme is) One large part of the phonology of a language concerns the orgranization of segments into larger objects: syllables. Syllable structure is relevant both for generalizations about segment distributions and for generalizations about stress (and related phenomena) in a language

The Syllable A familiar notion is that of the syllable: as in, ‘Philadelphia’ has five syllables A refined set of hypothesis about the syllable is important for many linguistic generalizations Definitions (initial): Onset: the beginning of the syllable Nucleus: vowel in the middle of the syllable Coda: consonant(s) at the end of a syllable As we will see in the next slide, these three do not count the same

Syllable Structure A single syllable’s (s) structure: Monosyllabic cat: s Onset Rhyme k Nucleus Coda æ t

Comments Each of the different components of the syllable may be relevant for different generalizations (what goes into an onset versus what can appear in the coda) The nucleus and the coda form a unit together that excludes the onset The motivation for this arrangement has to do with the fact that onsets appear to be irrelevant for some aspects of syllable structure (e.g. nuclei and codas count together for syllable weight) Just focus on the different parts for the moment, not the motivation for this internal arrangement

Onsets and Speech Errors Spoonerisms (Rev. Dr. W. A. Spooner, 1844-1930) Target: dear old queen Output: queer old dean Target: You have wasted the whole term Output: You have tasted the whole worm. Target: You missed my history lectures. Output: You hissed my mystery lectures.

Pig Latin A language game showing sensitivity to structure: Hat > Athey Thin > Inthey Strong > Ongstrey Linguistics > Inguisticsley Apple > Appleey Pattern: Add a syllable to the end of the word; copy the onset of the word-initial syllable to the onset of the word-final; add ‘ey’ to the rhyme.

Further aspects of the syllable Onsets: English normally allows two consonants. [s] can be added initially in many cases as well, resulting in onsets with three consonants (e.g. splash) All sounds can occur in this position with the exception of [ng]. Thus the subdivision of the syllable is crucial for stating this generalization. The Nucleus: Sometimes a consonant can function as a nucleus, like in kitten. The coda: English normally allows two consonants, although again there are cases where more stack up (e.g. belts) Sometimes the morphology produces something that is hard to pronounce: consider cases like the pronunciation of 3/8

Syllables and well-formedness Conditions on syllable structure define a set of (phonologically) possible words in a language; for instance: Actual words, obstruent + liquid: brick, true, free, crab, etc. Non-words that are possible words of English: blick, clee, flork Impossible words, e.g. obstruent + nasal: *bnick, *fnee, *dmay Compare: words in which historical change has made an initial consonant silent: knee, knight, gnat

Differences bzdura "nonsense" Languages differ in terms of the constraints they impose on syllable structure: E.g. Hawaiian: No coda consonants Maximum of one consonant per onset Examples: ink > 'înika Norman > Nolemana E.g. Polish: many consonant clusters at the beginning of words that are impossible in English: bzdura "nonsense" babsk "witch" grzbiet [gzhbyet] "back" marnotrawstw [mar-no-trafstf] "of wastes"

Metrical Structure Illustrating our tacit knowledge of abstract properties of language. English Stress: indePENdent twenty-SEven LEgislator MissisSIppi

Metrical structure, cont. An active topic in phonological research concerns the stress patterns of words. It appears that the world’s languages group syllables into feet, like the iamb and the trochee: Iamb: (’) Trochee (’) You will have seen something similar before in the study of metrical verse (poetry); one member of the foot is prominent (stressed). Languages differ in terms of what types of feet they use, whether they involve iterative feet throughout a word, and so on. English stress turns out to be sort of complicated for stress for a variety of reasons, but we can see some aspects of foot structure nevertheless.

The Rhythm Rule Remember those words we saw above? TWEnty-seven MISsissippi LEgislators I.e., without having been taught this rule, speakers automatically calculate a shift in stress Not a rule like that coming from English teachers (e.g. “No split-infinitives”); part of what speakers know unconsciously Consider e.g. thirteen Japanese bamboo tables

Expletive Infixation We’ll do morphology next week; for fun now: Suffix: Attached to the end of a word (work-ed) Prefix: Beginning (un-important) Infix: Inside a word What is an example of an infix in English? There is at least one phenomenon with the relevant properties.

Patterns Expletive Infixation is not something that our English teachers instruct us in; yet we know a great deal about it: 1) *fanta-f*cking-stic fan-f*cking-tastic 2) unrea-f*cking-listic *unreali-f*cking-stic *unre-f*cking-alistic *indepen-f*cking-dent inde-f*cking-pendent

Abstract Metrical Structure Stress placement involves computation over abstract representations, and these define expletive infixation. Recall: grouping into feet; one syllable in the foot receives prominence (stress): E.g. Àppalàchicóla Has three binary feet (s s): (Àppa)(làchi)(cóla) In which the first member of each foot is prominent.

The Rule The placement of the expletive is governed by the following condition: Expletive Insertion: The expletive is inserted immediately before a non-initial stress foot of the word: (Appa)(lachi)(cola) 1 2 Has two such junctures; so either Appa-f*cking-lachicola, or Appalachi-f*cking-cola (Disclaimer: the rule is actually more complicated…)