Chapter 21 Experience-dependent Plasticity Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 4: The Visual Cortex and Beyond
Advertisements

The Primary Visual Cortex
Activity-Dependent Development I April 23, 2007 Mu-ming Poo 1.Development of OD columns 2.Effects of visual deprivation 3. The critical period 4. Hebb’s.
Chapter 6 The Visual System
Blue= rods Green = Cones Pathways from the Retina In the brain, retinal ganglion axons travel to… –the hypothalamus: control bodily rhythms.
F behavioral plasticity F CH8: development of learning in songbirds F behavioral analysis of birdsong F singing in the brain F summary PART 4: BEHAVIORAL.
1 Activity-Dependent Development Plasticity 1.Development of OD columns 2.Effects of visual deprivation 3. The critical period 4. Hebb’s hypothesis 5.
Higher Processing of Visual Information: Lecture I --- April 2, 2007 by Mu-ming Poo 1.Overview of the Mammalian Visual System 2.Structure of Lateral Geniculate.
Color vision Different cone photo- receptors have opsin molecules which are differentially sensitive to certain wavelengths of light – these are the physical.
Plasticity in sensory systems Jan Schnupp on the monocycle.
Critical periods A time period when environmental factors have especially strong influence in a particular behavior. –Language fluency –Birds- Are you.
Cell Types and Physiology in the CANS. Major Components of the Central Auditory Nervous System (CANS) VIIIth cranial nerve Cochlear Nucleus Superior Olivary.
Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) 1.Overview of central visual pathway 2.Projection from retina to LGN 3.LGN layers: P and M pathways 4.LGN receptive fields.
Critical periods in development - “nature” vs. “nurture”
Sensory systems basics. Sensing the external world.
THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EYE TO CORTEX Outline 1. The Eyes a. Structure b. Accommodation c. Binocular Disparity 2. The Retina a. Structure b. Completion c. Cone.
The Visual Cortex: Anatomy
Visual Neuroscience Visual pathways Retina Photoreceptors Rods Cones Spacing and density ON and OFF pathways Ganglion cells Retinal projections SCN Pretectum.
Chapter 3: Neural Processing and Perception. Neural Processing and Perception Neural processing is the interaction of signals in many neurons.
Bird Song & hearing Lecture 1 Aim nOutline the physiology of hearing and vocalisations u seasonal variation u developmental processes u the way.
Auditory Neuroscience 1 Spatial Hearing Systems Biology Doctoral Training Program Physiology course Prof. Jan Schnupp HowYourBrainWorks.net.
1 Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Chapter 19 Visual Network Moran Furman.
Key points Early abnormal stimulation/activity leads to long-term functional deficits (in vision these deficits may be called amblyopias) Understand key.
Activity-dependent Development
Sensory Neural Systems 5 February 2008 Rachel L. León
$ studying barn owls in the laboratory $ sound intensity cues $ sound timing cues $ neural pathways for sound location $ auditory space $ interaural time.
1 Perception and VR MONT 104S, Spring 2008 Lecture 3 Central Visual Pathways.
Organization of Song Learning
Volume 54, Issue 6, Pages (June 2007)
The Geometry of Visual Cortical Maps
Sharif Taha, Michael P. Stryker  Neuron 
Learning to Like Your Voice
Volume 19, Issue 2, Pages (August 1997)
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Volume 48, Issue 5, Pages (December 2005)
Rhea R. Kimpo, Allison J. Doupe  Neuron 
The Generation of Direction Selectivity in the Auditory System
Masayuki Haruta, Yoshio Hata  Current Biology 
Volume 48, Issue 2, Pages (October 2005)
Ben Scholl, Xiang Gao, Michael Wehr  Neuron 
Hongbo Yu, Brandon J. Farley, Dezhe Z. Jin, Mriganka Sur  Neuron 
Bartlett D. Moore, Caitlin W. Kiley, Chao Sun, W. Martin Usrey  Neuron 
A Map for Horizontal Disparity in Monkey V2
Jonathan J. Nassi, David C. Lyon, Edward M. Callaway  Neuron 
Development and Plasticity of the Primary Visual Cortex
Sleep Enhances Plasticity in the Developing Visual Cortex
Katherine M. Armstrong, Jamie K. Fitzgerald, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Michele M Solis, Allison J Doupe  Neuron 
Volume 57, Issue 4, Pages (February 2008)
Why Seeing Is Believing: Merging Auditory and Visual Worlds
Evolution of sound localisation in land vertebrates
Origin and Dynamics of Extraclassical Suppression in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of the Macaque Monkey  Henry J. Alitto, W. Martin Usrey  Neuron  Volume.
Daniel E. Winkowski, Eric I. Knudsen  Neuron 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Bor-Shuen Wang, Rashmi Sarnaik, Jianhua Cang  Neuron 
Neuronal Plasticity: Beyond the Critical Period
Volume 24, Issue 8, Pages e6 (August 2018)
Dynamic Shape Synthesis in Posterior Inferotemporal Cortex
Volume 46, Issue 1, Pages 4-7 (April 2005)
Basal Forebrain Cholinergic Input Is Not Essential for Lesion-Induced Plasticity in Mature Auditory Cortex  Marc R. Kamke, Mel Brown, Dexter R.F. Irvine 
Outline Announcements Human Visual Information Processing
Visual development: Making maps in the dark
Construction of Complex Receptive Fields in Cat Primary Visual Cortex
The superior colliculus
Rhea R. Kimpo, Allison J. Doupe  Neuron 
Vision and Cortical Map Development
Ocular dominance maps in an animal in which the hemisphere contralateral to the deprived eye had been injected with chondroitinase. Ocular dominance maps.
Neuronal Plasticity: Beyond the Critical Period
Cristopher M. Niell, Stephen J Smith  Neuron 
Sarah C. Woolley, Raghav Rajan, Mati Joshua, Allison J. Doupe  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

Chapter 21 Experience-dependent Plasticity Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

FIGURE 21.1 Songs of white-crowned sparrows. These are sonograms (time-frequency sound spectrograms) of songs from birds with different kinds of early experience. Sound energy in each frequency band is indicated by the darkness of the trace. (A) Song dialects. Birds raised in different areas sing slightly different songs. These dialects are stable for many years and are transmitted by learning. (B) Isolate songs. These simpler songs develop in birds raised in acoustic isolation or in birds that fail to copy a tutor song. (C) Songs of deafened birds. These kinds of songs develop in birds that are deafened after the sensitive period for song memorization but before the period of vocal learning. The birds need to hear their own voice to develop normal song. From Konishi (1985).

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.2 The sensitive period for song memorization and the set of nuclei that comprise the song system in songbirds. (A) Time-line for song learning for the white-crowned sparrow. For this species, the sensitive period for song memorization does not overlap with the period when the bird learns to sing. (B) A schematic diagram of a side view of the brain of a songbird. The vocal motor pathway, shown in red, consists of the HVC, the robust nucleus of the archopallium (RA), and the hypoglossal nucleus. The anterior basal ganglia pathway, shown in blue, consists of Area X, the medial portion of the dorsolateral nucleus of the thalamus (DLM), and the lateral portion of the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN).

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.3 The ascending auditory pathway to the optic tectumin the barn owl. Auditory inputs enter the optic tectum from the brainstem (bottomarrows); these inputs already encode frequency-specific information about interaural time difference (ITD). These inputs project into the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus (ICC), where they are organized topographically by frequency. Bands in the ICC represent these frequencies (e.g., 4, 6, 8 kHz). Neurons in the ICC convey information both to the auditory thalamus (the primary pathway) and to the external nucleus of the inferior colliculus (ICX). In the ICX, ITD information is combined across frequency channels to synthesize a map of auditory space. For example, an ITD of 0 μs is generated by sound stimuli directly in front of the animal, such that sound reaches the two ears at exactly the same time. At the position marked 0 μs in the ICX are neurons that respond maximally to sounds with an ITD value of 0 μs and thus respond selectively to sounds originating in front of the animal. Sounds originating, for example, from positions that are further to the left-hand side will reach the ears with progressively greater left ear-leading ITDs and thus stimulate neurons with progressively larger best ITDs. From the ICX, the auditory map of space is conveyed via a topographic projection to the optic tectum. Here the auditory map is aligned and merged with a visual map of space (top arrows, representing inputs from the retina and the forebrain) to produce a multimodal space map.

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.4 Rearing owls with laterally displacing, optical prisms causes an adaptive shift in the tuning of neurons in the optic tectum for ITD. (A) This map represents the space in front of the owl, showing both the elevation and the azimuth of a stimulus in space. Contour lines indicate the correspondence of ITD values (in microseconds) with particular locations in space. The point at which the 0° axes intersect represents the point in space directly in front of the owl’s head. The auditory (A) and visual (V) receptive fields of one tectal neuron are shown in the center of the map. This neuron responds optimally when the stimulus is directly in front of the animal. Normally, the auditory and visual receptive fields are aligned. Optical prisms induce a horizontal displacement of the neuron’s visual receptive field (VRF), resulting in a misalignment between A and V. (B) Tuning for ITD is shifted by prism experience. These ITD tuning curves were recorded from similar sites in the optic tectum before (blue) and after (purple) 8 weeks of prism experience. Both sites had a VRF at 0° azimuth. After 8 weeks of experience, the neuron is tuned for the ITD produced by an acoustic stimulus at the location of the optically displaced VRF, as shown in A. Arrows indicate the best ITD for each site; the best ITD is defined as the center of the range of ITDs to which the neuron responded with more than 50% of its maximum response. (C) The relationship between best ITD and VRF azimuth is shifted systematically from normal in prism-reared owls. The black line indicates the regression of best ITD on VRF azimuth that is observed in normal owls. Dots represent individual sites in a prism-reared owl. The map of ITD is shifted systematically relative to the visual map of space.

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.5 The sensitive period for visual calibration of neuronal ITD tuning in the optic tectum. Each dot represents data from a single owl. The large arrow below is a timeline, indicating important developmental stages in an owl ’ s life. Each owl experienced a 23° displacement of the visual field for at least 60 days. ITD tuning was then measured at 15 to 23 sites in the superficial layers of the optic tectum. The difference between the best ITD measured and the best ITD expected normally, based on the location of the site ’ s VRF (see Fig. 21.4C), was taken as the “ shift in ITD tuning. ” The mean shift in ITD tuning for the population of sampled sites as a function of the age of the owl when prisms were first mounted is plotted.

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.6 Schematic model of the change in the pattern of axonal projections from the ICC to the ICX that accompanies the shift in the map of ITD in the ICX. Based on DeBello et al. (2001). (A) The initial state of the projection before prism experience. (B) After prism experience, axonal projections from the ICC to the ICX are shifted systematically as indicated by the red arbors.

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.7 Critical period plasticity of the anatomical projections representing the left and right eyes in the visual cortex of monkeys. These are dark field autoradiographs of tangential sections through layer 4 of the primary visual cortex, showing the pattern of trans-synaptic labeling (bright areas) from the thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) that resulted from an injection of a radioactive tracer into one eye; the intervening dark areas represent LGN input from the opposite (non-injected) eye. Scale bar indicates 2.5 mm. (A) The pattern of LGN input to the visual cortex in a normal monkey. (B) Severely reduced LGN input from the deprived eye of a monkey that experienced monocular deprivation from 3 weeks until 7 months old. (C) Greatly expanded LGN input from the nondeprived eye of a monkey that experienced monocular deprivation from 2 weeks until 18 months old. From Hubel et al. (1977).

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.8 Effect of chronic closure of one eye on the responsiveness of visual cortical neurons to input from each eye. (A) Ocular dominance distribution in the primary visual cortex of two normal kittens, 3 to 4 weeks old. Cells in group 1 were driven only by the contralateral eye; for group 2, the contralateral eye was markedly dominant; for group 3, the contralateral eye was slightly dominant; for group 4, there was no apparent difference in the drive from the two eyes; for group 5, the ipsilateral eye dominated slightly; for group 6, it dominated markedly; and for group 7, cells were driven only by the ipsilateral eye. (B) Ocular dominance distribution was altered dramatically in a kitten exposed to contralateral eye closure for 1 week (from 23 to 29 days of age). (C) Ocular dominance distribution was essentially normal in an adult cat exposed to contralateral eye closure for 26 months. From Hubel and Wiesel (1970).

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE 21.9 The critical period for binocular processing in the primary visual cortex of the cat. The degree of functional disconnection of cortical neurons from the deprived eye is quantified and plotted as a function of the kitten ’ s age at the time of monocular closure. Chronic monocular closure lasted 10 to 12 days. Each point represents data from a single animal. Functional disconnection was based on the ocular dominance distribution (see Fig. 21.8) and indicated the degree to which the influence of the closed eye was weakened or lost. The index was defined such that the mean value for normal cats was 0, whereas total disconnection resulted in a value of 1. From Olson and Freeman (1980).

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. FIGURE A rat mother grooms her pups during the first week after their birth. This interaction permanently affects the temperament of these rat pups.