D OES R EARING E NVIRONMENT S HAPE R OMANTIC A TTACHMENT S TYLE ? U SING S IBLINGS TO T EST FOR S HARED E NVIRONMENTAL I NFLUENCES Amy E. Johnson, Bethany.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The links between sibling relationships and the whole family
Advertisements

Children’s subjective well-being Findings from national surveys in England International Society for Child Indicators Conference, 27 th July 2011.
AGE VARIATION IN MATING STRATEGIES AND MATE PREFERENCES AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS Danielle Ryan and April Bleske-Rechek, University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire.
The Journey Of Adulthood, 6/e Helen L. Bee & Barbara R. Bjorklund Chapter 6 SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS.
Attachment Theory.
Adolescent Attachment to Parents: Predicting Later Adolescent Rejection Sensitivity I would like to thank the William T. Grant Foundation, Spencer Foundation,
B IRTH O RDER AND P ERSONALITY : T ESTING A SSUMPTIONS WITH I NDEPENDENT S IBLINGS ’ R EPORTS Bethany R. Franklin, Amy E. Johnson, & Jenna A. Kelley University.
Predictors and Consequences of Involvement in Age-Discrepant Romantic Relationships Amy Bender Hanover College 2007.
Marital Satisfaction and Family Functioning in Families with Toddlers: Evidence For a Single Construct? Phillip R. Sevigny, M. A. & Lynn Loutzenhiser,
1]Knoll, N., Burkert, S., & Schwartzer, R. (2006). Reciprocal support provision: Personality as a moderator? European Journal of Personality, 20,
Lindsay Chase-Lansdale, Andrew Cherlin and Kathleen Kiernan
Introduction Method  Evaluation of ability to provide social support yielded scores with good internal consistency reliability.  There was moderate agreement.
Genetic Factors Predisposing to Homosexuality May Increase Mating Success in Heterosexuals Written by Zietsch et. al By Michael Berman and Lindsay Tooley.
Implications of research into attachment and day care
Adolescent Romantic Relationships: The Impact of Rejection Sensitivity and the Moderating Role of Attachment Security By: Emily Marston, Amanda Hare, Erin.
College Students' Attitudes Toward and Knowledge of Disabilities: Does Religiosity Matter? Kristi Kluegel and Olivia Tomfohrde, Faculty Advisor: Mary Beth.
Attraction and Flirtation in Young Adults’ and Middle-Aged Adults’ Opposite-Sex Friendships Erin E. Hirsch, Cierra A. Micke, and April Bleske-Rechek University.
Environmentalism and Personality Simon Cohen Jeff Edgar Chris Latham-Warde Sanni Kujala.
Longitudinal Research methods in personality psychology.
Exploring College Students’ Perceptions of Their Peers With Disabilities Katie Beck a and Kellie Risberg a, Faculty Mentor: Mary Beth Leibham b, Ph.D.
Trait Resilience and Distress Among Family Caregivers of Children with Severe Disabilities Receiving Pulmonary Services Courtney Francis 1, Ryan T. Blucker.
Personality Psychology Brent W. Roberts University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
A TTRACTION AND A TTRACTIVENESS IN M ALE -F EMALE D YADS : D O D ATING C OUPLES D IFFER FROM C ROSS -S EX F RIENDS ? Whitney Joseph, Heather Williquette,
Figure 1. Display of the percentage of men and women in each age group who spontaneously nominated mating desires as a benefit of their friendships (left)
Adolescent Romantic Relationships and Depressive Symptoms: The Moderating Effect of Emotional Intelligence Introduction David E. Szwedo
B IRTH O RDER AND P ERSONALITY : A W ITHIN -F AMILY T EST OF C ULTURAL L ORE Jenna A. Kelley University of Wisconsin – Eau Claire, Department of Psychology.
Social and Emotional Costs of “One-Sided Friendships” in Adolescence. Lauren Molloy & Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
Social Cognitive & Trait Theories
Module 20 Social Cognitive & Trait Theories. SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY Definition –says that personality development is shaped primarily by three forces:
Why do babies develop attachments? Lesson Four. Starter: The Continuity Hypothesis The types of relationships people have later on in their lives will.
Friends as Rivals: Perceptions of Attractiveness Predict Mating Rivalry in Female Friendships Stephanie R. A. Maves, Sarah L. Hubert, and April Bleske-Rechek.
Ethnic Identity among Mexican American Adolescents: The Role of Maternal Cultural Values and Parenting Practices 1 Miriam M. Martinez, 1 Gustavo Carlo,
Problem-Solving Abilities and Feelings of Control: A Work in Progress Emily M. Kaiser, Department of Communication Studies, College of Arts and Sciences.
Dyadic Sex Differences in Adoptive and Nonadoptive Family Relationships during Emerging Adulthood Amy Walkner-Spaan, M.S.W., L.I.S.W. Martha Rueter, Ph.D.
Attractive Equals Smart? Perceived Intelligence as a Function of Attractiveness and Gender Abstract Method Procedure Discussion Participants were 38 men.
Module 13 Intelligence. INTRODUCTION Psychometrics –Subarea of psychology –Concerned with developing psychological tests that assess an individual’s abilities,
Dads Do Matter: Adolescents’ Relationships With Dads Predicting Later Rejection Sensitivity I would like to thank the William T. Grant Foundation, Spencer.
The Heritability of Religiousness: An International Twin Study Amy E. Steffes University of Wisconsin – Eau Claire Faculty Mentor: April Bleske-Rechek.
Expecting the worst often leads to poor outcomes. This process is particularly true in close relationships, as those who are most sensitive to rejection.
Session 6 Genetic Influence on Behaviour. What do attached ear lobes, blue eyes, and tongue-rolling have in common?
1 Human Behavior and the Social Environment: Theories for Social Work Practice Bruce A. Thyer, Catherine N. Dulmus, and Karen M. Sowers, Editors Chapter.
BACKGROUND Attachment  Bowlby’s (1973) attachment theory states children form strong affectionate bonds with their primary caregiver. Ainsworth & Wittig.
The purpose of this research project was to find a correlation between concordance rates for communication disorders in monozygotic and dizygotic twins.
BECOMING AN ADULT Transition to Adulthood Continued…
Parents' Marital Functioning and the Development of Adolescent Romantic Relationships Amanda L. Hare, F. Christy McFarland, & Joseph P. Allen University.
Overview: In South Africa, the Child and Youth Care Worker (CYCW) provides care to children and families impacted by HIV/AIDS. The job duties of a CYCW.
Attachment in Adulthood Simpson, J. S., & Rholes, W. S. (1998). Attachment in adulthood. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and.
Born First, Born Smarter? Zajonc, R.B., & Markus, G.B. (1975) Birth order and intellectual development. Psychological Review,
The authors would like to acknowledge the families at the Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin Jane P. Pettit Pain and Palliative Care Center. For more information,
Relationship and Friendship Satisfaction Mary Kate Smith and Nicole Malott Penn State University – York, PA Abstract Introduction References We were interested.
Figure 1. Family Study 1. Breakdown of Attachment Category, by Subgroup Figure 2. Family Study 2. Breakdown of Attachment Category, by Subgroup Category.
Parental, Temperament, & Peer Influences on Disordered Eating Symptoms Kaija M. Muhich, Alyssa Collura, Jessica Hick and Jennifer J. Muehlenkamp Psychology.
B ACKGROUND M ETHOD D ISCUSSION R EFERENCES R ESULTS A CKNOWLEDGMENTS S IMILARITY B ETWEEN F RIENDS AND R OMANTIC P ARTNERS IN M ORAL I NTUITIONS Mallory.
Attempt to explain how changes occur Each new generation of animals shows some genetic variation from the previous one. If any such variations enhance.
ATTACHMENT THEORY AND THE KEY PERSON APPROACH
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
Florida International University, Miami, FL
Investigating Self-Selection Bias in Mindfulness Research
Attraction and Attractiveness in a Naturally Occurring
The relationship between childhood and later life Matt Jarvis
Brotherson, S., Kranzler, B., & Zehnacker, G.
University of Wisconsin – Eau Claire  Department of Psychology
Krystle Lange & Regan A. R. Gurung University of Wisconsin, Green Bay
Psychological origins of attraction
Research-Based Answers to Frequently Asked Questions About: Remarriage
Adult relationships.
In pairs complete the Agony Aunt task
Influence of early attachment on later relationships
The Nature-Nurture Debates
Attachment, caregiving and parenting: A mediational model Presented at the International Association for Relationships Research Conference, July 2008,
Presentation transcript:

D OES R EARING E NVIRONMENT S HAPE R OMANTIC A TTACHMENT S TYLE ? U SING S IBLINGS TO T EST FOR S HARED E NVIRONMENTAL I NFLUENCES Amy E. Johnson, Bethany R. Franklin, and Jenna A. Kelley University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire, Psychology Department (Faculty Mentor: April Bleske-Rechek) Study 1 Introduction Method Acknowledgments Select References Discussion Attachment research began when John Bowlby recognized the importance of the bonds, or “attachments,” formed between infants and their caregivers. In the late 1980s, psychologists extended Bowlby’s research into the domain of adult romantic love by proposing that romantic love can be conceptualized as a process of becoming attached (e.g., Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Relationship scientists documented that individuals differ in two primary attachment dimensions: avoidance, or the extent to which individuals are uncomfortable with closeness, and anxiety, the extent to which individuals worry about abandonment in their relationships (Feeney, Noller, & Callan, 1994). Since Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) initial conceptualization of romantic love as an attachment process, researchers have documented links between attachment dimensions and memories of one’s relationships with parents during childhood, parental relationship status (e.g., divorced), and one’s own current and past romantic relationship experiences. Research has not, however, determined the causes of individual differences in romantic attachment style. We conducted a series of studies to explore the possibility that romantic attachment style is transmitted via the family, either through shared genes or shared rearing environment. Family members should be similar in attachment style if it is transmitted familially. In Study 1, we investigated similarity in attachment dimensions among 180 adult children and their parents. In Study 2, we again investigated similarity among 169 adult children and their parents; we also collected data from 52 siblings to determine if being reared in the same home induces similarity between siblings’ romantic attachment styles. In Study 3, we collected attachment and personality data from 78 young adult sibling pairs. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp ). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, Fraley, R. C. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4, Study 1: Young Adults and Their Parents A total of 183 young adults (46 men, 137 women) participated. They completed a 36-item attachment questionnaire that provides scores on continuous dimensions of anxiety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Sample items on the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) inventory include “I worry about being abandoned by my romantic partners” (anxiety) and “I try to avoid getting close to my partners” (avoidance). Participants provided their responses on a seven-point rating scale (strongly disagree to strongly agree). Internal reliabilities in our samples exceeded.90. We mailed a blank version of the ECR to each participant’s mom and/or dad. Forty-five percent of fathers and 65% of mothers returned their questionnaires, resulting in 87 child- father pairs and 120 child-mother pairs. Study 2: Young Adults and Their Parents and Siblings A new sample of 169 pairs of young adults and their parent(s) participated in our second study. To augment the previous dataset, we mailed the ECR to one sibling of each participant (if they had a sibling of 18 years or older). A total of 52 siblings sent in questionnaires. In addition, participants completed the brief but internally valid Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI). This measure was used to establish the validity of our sample. Study 3: Young Adults and Their Siblings Our third sample included 92 young adults (22 men, 70 women) and 78 of their siblings who completed the ECR as well as the 44-item Big Five Inventory. Again, personality measures were included to establish the validity of our sample. We thank all the participants who generously permitted us to invite their family members to participate in the study and those who encouraged their siblings to complete their online questionnaires in a timely fashion. We also thank faculty who allowed us into their classrooms to recruit and run participants: Dr. Jeffrey Goodman, Andrew Hucks, Dr. Kevin Klatt, Dr. Mary Beth Leibham, and Dr. Jennifer Muehlenkamp. This research was supported by funding from the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs at the University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire. In Study 2, we again investigated similarity between adult children and their parents. We also included data from 52 of the young adults’ siblings. Similarity between siblings would support the hypothesis that being raised in the same home (in a similar family environment) induces similarity in romantic attachment style. Pair-wise associations showed no significant similarity between family members, all rs.16. The results for sibling pairs are displayed at right and left. The results of Studies 1 and 2 illustrated that parents’ attitudes toward their romantic relationships, as assessed by the Experiences in Close Relationships inventory, are not similar to their children’s attitudes and thus are not transmitted to them via either role model or shared genes. In trying to interpret this lack of similarity between adult children and their parents, we observed that the parents in Studies 1 and 2 scored relatively low overall in anxiety and avoidance. Other studies have documented that people in long-standing relationships (as most of these parents were) tend to score low in avoidance and anxiety. Associations can be difficult to detect when there is restriction of range. Thus, in Studies 2 and 3 we utilized siblings to test for the familial transmission of attachment style with samples that would be more likely to vary widely in their attachment scores. Again, however, the weight of the evidence from Studies 2 and 3 suggests little similarity between family members. Taken together, our findings suggest that a substantial proportion of individual differences in romantic attachment style are due to differences in non-shared environmental influences (such as individuals’ unique relationship histories) as opposed to genetic differences or shared rearing environments. The non-significant associations we found do not appear to be a product of biased sampling because our samples functioned much like previous samples in several ways: (1) In Studies 1 and 2 the parents were more secure than were the young adults, and in Studies 2 and 3 the young men were less anxious than the young women; (2) in Studies 1 and 2, romantic partners (mothers and fathers) were not similar to each other in their attachment scores; (3) in Studies 2 and 3, individuals who scored higher in attachment anxiety also scored higher in the personality trait Neuroticism; (4) in Study 2, family members showed weak but positive similarity in the primary personality traits of Neuroticism and Extraversion; and in Study 3, family members showed weak-to-moderate similarity in the primary personality traits of Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and Openness. Study 2 Study 3 Study 1: Sample Descriptive Statistics Young Adult Men (n=46) Young Adult Women (n=137) Fathers (n=85-86) Mothers (n= ) Attachment Avoidance 2.87 (0.83)2.70 (0.94)2.49 (0.85)2.42 (1.00) Attachment Anxiety 3.45 (0.76)3.74 (0.92)3.00 (0.94)2.79 (1.05) In Study 1, we investigated similarity between adult children and their parents’ attachment styles. Similarity would support the hypothesis that the parents’ own relationship functioning is transmitted to children either through shared environment or shared genes. Contrary to that hypothesis, young adults were not similar to their parents in either attachment anxiety or attachment avoidance, all rs.28. The results for child-mother pairs are displayed at left and right; the results with fathers were identical. Study 2: Sample Descriptive Statistics Young Adults (n=43M, 125F) Young Adults’ Siblings (n=17M, 35F) Fathers (n=71-72) Mothers (n=89-92) Attachment Avoidance 2.81 (0.93)2.87 (1.03)2.53 (0.86)2.59 (1.01) Attachment Anxiety 3.62 (0.96)3.84 (0.99)3.04 (0.96)2.90 (1.10) Study 3: Sample Descriptive Statistics Young Adult Men (n=22) Young Adult Women (n=70) Young Adults’ Brothers (n=31) Young Adults’ Sisters (n=47) Attachment Avoidance 2.73 (1.12)2.55 (0.96)2.87 (1.10)2.67 (1.01) Attachment Anxiety 3.66 (0.99)3.85 (1.02)3.61 (0.89)3.70 (1.28) In Study 3, we had attachment data from 78 pairs of siblings, again testing the hypothesis that sharing a rearing environment induces similarity in romantic attachment style. As in Study 2, siblings were not similar in avoidance, r(78) =.10, p =.379; however, siblings were weakly to moderately similar in attachment anxiety, r(78) =.25, p =.026. Further, the association held at r =.28 after controlling for siblings’ neuroticism, a personality trait which is consistently associated with experiencing attachment anxiety.