Grouping David Meredith Aalborg University. Musical grouping structure Listeners automatically chunk or “segment” music into structural units of various.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 2: Rhythm and Pitch
Advertisements

Chapter 5: Space and Form Form & Pattern Perception: Humans are second to none in processing visual form and pattern information. Our ability to see patterns.
Auditory scene analysis 2
Perception. Rules of Perceptual Organization Gestalt Psychologists The whole is more than the sum of its parts Closure Even if there are gaps in a picture.
Perception Chapter 4.
Identifying Frequencies. Terms: LoudnessPitchTimbre.
Melodic Organization Chapter 6. Motive Short melodic and/or rhythmic pattern Usually only a few beats Recurs throughout a piece or section Unifying element.
Music Perception. Why music perception? 1. Found in all cultures - listening to music is a universal activity. 2. Interesting from a developmental point.
GROUP MEMBERS-  ZION- PROJECT LEADER  TYRESE-CHIEF RESEARCHER  MUSKAN-COMMUNICATIONS DIRECTOR  GHAZAL-DIGITAL ENGINEER.
Auditory Scene Analysis (ASA). Auditory Demonstrations Albert S. Bregman / Pierre A. Ahad “Demonstration of Auditory Scene Analysis, The perceptual Organisation.
Gestalt Principles Gurpreet. What are the Gestalt Principles? Gestalt is a psychological term which means ‘unified whole’. It was discovered by the German.
Chapter 6 Melodic Organization.
Excursions in Modern Mathematics, 7e: Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. 16 Mathematics of Normal Distributions 16.1Approximately Normal.
A.Diederich – International University Bremen – USC – MMM – Spring 2005 Rhythm and timing  Clarke, E.F. Rhythm and timing in music. In Deutsch, D. Chapter.
Principles of Design Can You Name The Principles of Design?
Timbre (pronounced like: Tamber) pure tones are very rare a single note on a musical instrument is a superposition (i.e. several things one on top of.
Effects of Metrical Subdivision on Perceived Beat Rate Bruno H. Repp Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut.
What are harmonics? Superposition of two (or more) frequencies yields a complex wave with a fundamental frequency.
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 2 – Perception.
Streaming David Meredith Aalborg University. Sequential integration The connection of parts of an auditory spectrum over time to form concurrent streams.
GCSE Psychology Gestalt Laws.
Introduction to algorithmic models of music cognition David Meredith Aalborg University.
Metre David Meredith Aalborg University. Theories of musical metre A theory of musical metre should be able to predict – points in time when listeners.
HANA HARRISON CSE 435 NOVEMBER 19, 2012 Music Composition.
Streaming David Meredith Aalborg University. Sequential integration The connection of parts of an auditory spectrum over time to form concurrent streams.
Audio Scene Analysis and Music Cognitive Elements of Music Listening
By Andrea Rees. Gestalt Principles 1) Closure 2) Proximity 3) Similarity 4) Figure VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES OVERVIEW Depth Principles Binocular 1)
Five principles of design (in fast forward) You can find this and other helpful PowerPoints on my teacher web site at Hillsboro R-3 under teacher web sites.
Reading ACT Test. Format 40 questions/4 passages/35 minutes/ ½ minutes per passage 2-3 minutes to read each passage and 5-6 to answer questions.
NEW MODEL, OLD PROBLEM: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION INTO GROUPING AND METRICAL CONSTRAINTS IN MUSIC PERCEPTION NEW MODEL, OLD PROBLEM: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION.
Chapter 5: Spatial Vision & Form Perception
Melodic Organization Motive Rhythmic Motive Melodic Motive
Perception Chapter 5.
CHAPTER 4 – SENSATION AND PERCEPTION SECTION 1 – SENSATION AND PERCEPTION: THE BASICS Objective: DISTINGUISH BETWEEN SENSATION AND PERCEPTION, AND EXPLAIN.
The Principles of Design
ARTDIRECTION BASIC DESIGN PRINCIPLES. PRINCIPLES OF DESIGN The combination of design elements.
Warm Up #8 How many pegs do you see?
Chapter 3 Scales and Melody.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
The use of elements in a work of art to create a consistent effect All parts of a work of art working together to create a consistent effect – no one.
Principles of Grouping & Pitch Theory By Kayla and Anika.
Set 7 What’s that scale?? 1 Note Grades should be available on some computer somewhere. The numbers are based on the total number of correct answers,
A Compression-Based Model of Musical Learning David Meredith DMRN+7, Queen Mary University of London, 18 December 2012.
Visual Perception Principles Visual perception principles are ‘rules’ that we apply to visual information to assist our organisation and interpretation.
Other Aspects of Musical Sound pp Texture  Texture describes the number of things that are going on at once in a piece of music.  Monophony-
Things to Consider When Writing Melodies Vital Elements  Two most vital elements - rhythm and melody.  Harmonic structure of your composition will.
Visual Organization Module 19.
Timbre and Memory An experiment for the musical mind Emily Yang Yu Music 151, 2008.
1 HETEROSCEDASTICITY: WEIGHTED AND LOGARITHMIC REGRESSIONS This sequence presents two methods for dealing with the problem of heteroscedasticity. We will.
Visual Perception. What is Visual Perception? Visual perception are rules we apply to visual information to assist our organisation and interpretation.
HOLT, RINEHART AND WINSTON P SYCHOLOGY PRINCIPLES IN PRACTICE 1 Chapter 4 Rules of Perceptual Organization Perception—the way in which we organize or make.
Discovering Musical Patterns through Perceptive Heuristics By Oliver Lartillot Presentation by Ananda Jacobs.
VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES By Mikayla. VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES  Gestalt principles 1.Closure 2.Proximity 3.Similarity 4.Figure-ground  Depth.
GESTALT PRINCIPLES OF VISUAL DESIGN Gestalt principles of visual design are derived from the theories of gestalt psychology, which were applied to the.
Harmonic Models CS 275B/Music 254.
1. Rhythm 1.1. Basic rhythmsBasic rhythms 1.2. Rhythmic formulasRhythmic formulas 2. Melody 2.1. Diatonic scaleDiatonic scale Relative keys Degrees of.
Human Computer Interaction
Aim To test Cherry’s findings on attention ‘more rigorously’. Sample
Chapter 2: Rhythm and Pitch
Cognitive Processes PSY 334
The Bottom-Up Approach to Offender Profiling
Huxley’s Circle of Visual Perception
Perceiving and Recognizing Objects
Visual Rules Why are they necessary?
How you perceive your surroundings
Fine Arts section 1 pg.7-20 By david steen.
How your mind understands sensory information
Integrating Segmentation and Similarity in Melodic Analysis
Speech Perception (acoustic cues)
Volume 81, Issue 5, Pages (March 2014)
Presentation transcript:

Grouping David Meredith Aalborg University

Musical grouping structure Listeners automatically chunk or “segment” music into structural units of various sizes – e.g., motives, themes, phrases, sections, movements Such a structural unit is called a group (Lerdahl and Jackendoff, 1983, p.12) Some types of group indicated in scores – e.g., by breath marks, phrase marks, slurs Wind players often breathe at group boundaries Performers often slow down at the ends of larger groups Grouping structure is way that a piece is perceived to be segmented into structural units at various time scales

Musical grouping structure “Grouping can be viewed as the most basic component of musical understanding [because once a listener has] construed a grouping structure [he or she] has gone a long way towards ‘making sense’ of it” (Lerdahl and Jackendoff, 1983, p.13)

Musical grouping structure Listen to the fragment above The whole fragment forms a structural unit or group Naturally divides into two, equal-length phrases Each phrase divides into three ‘motives’ which are groups at a smaller level than the phrase How can we infer the grouping structure of a musical passage from its ‘surface’?

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s (1983) Generative Theory of Tonal Music Four interacting modules – Grouping structure: motives, themes, phrases, sections – Metrical structure: “hierarchical pattern of beats” – Time-span reduction: how some events elaborate or depend on other events – Prolongational reduction: the “ebb-and-flow of tension” Each module contains well-formedness rules and preference rules

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s (1983) theory of grouping structure Musical grouping is an “auditory analog of the partitioning of the visual field into objects, parts of objects, and parts of parts of objects” (p.36) Theory of grouping “seems to consist largely of general conditions for auditory pattern perception that have far broader application than for music alone” (p.36) “A listener needs to know relatively little about a musical idiom in order to assign grouping structure to pieces in that idiom” (p.36) Theory can only cope with homorhythmic music

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s grouping well-formedness rules Theory consists of 5 well-formedness rules, 7 preference rules and 2 transformational rules GWFR 1: “Any contiguous sequence of pitch-events, drum beats, or the like can constitute a group, and only contiguous sequences can constitute a group.” (p.37) – Means we can represent a group with a slur GWFR 2: “A piece constitutes a group.” (p.38) GWFR 3: “A group may contain smaller groups.” (p.38) – e.g., sections are split into phrases, which are split into motives

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s theory of grouping structure GWFR 4: “If a group G 1 contains part of a group G 2, then it must contain all of G 2 ” (p.38) – A higher-level group cannot start or end in the middle of a lower-level group A boundary between adjacent groups must also be a boundary between groups at lower structural levels – Adjacent groups in the same level cannot overlap In fact they can in certain special cases covered by the transformational rules

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s theory of grouping structure GWFR 5: “If a group G 1 contains a smaller group G 2, then G 1 must be exhaustively partitioned into smaller groups.” (p. 38) – The situation shown above cannot happen – If both A and B are group boundaries, then there must be a group that starts at A and ends at B A B

Well-formedness is not enough GWFRs are not enough to predict the grouping structure that we hear Need preference rules to isolate the well-formed analyses that correspond best to what we hear Both structures above are well-formed, but the lower one is certainly not what we hear – Can check this by playing with gaps at the group boundaries

Gestalt principles of proximity and similarity Principles governing musical grouping seem to be similar to those governing grouping in vision Gestalt theory (Wertheimer, 1938; Köhler, 1947; Koffka, 1935) proposes that grouping of elements in a visual scene governed by a small number of principles The principles of proximity and similarity seem to be operating in the perception of musical grouping structure

Gestalt principle of proximity In the left figure, we see a group of two circles on the left and 1 circle on the right In the middle figure, we see a single circle on the left and two circles on the right In the right figure, we see a group of three circles, with none separated more from the others In the left image, we do not, for example, see a single circle on the left and then a group of two circles with a large gap between them This suggests that elements are grouped according to proximity

Gestalt principle of similarity In the left figure, we see a group of three squares and a group of two circles In the right figure, we see a group of two squares and three circles In the left image, we do not, for example, see a group of two squares and a group containing two circles and a square Suggests that we group elements by similarity

Similarity and proximity in music Wertheimer (1938) himself observed that similarity and proximity seem to operate in music In A and B, proximity in time is operating – In A, the third note is heard to be grouped with the first two, whereas in B it is heard to be grouped with last two In C and D, similarity in pitch is operating – C is a group of 2 Fs followed by a group of 4 Cs – D is a group of 4 Fs followed by a group of 2 Cs ABCD

Gestalt principles can compete or reinforce each other On the left, proximity and similarity reinforce each other On the right, the principles compete, giving rise to ambiguity

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Local Detail Grouping Preference Rules GPR 1: “Avoid analyses with very small groups – the smaller, the less preferable.” (p.43) – A single event can only act as a group if it is strongly isolated or functions as a motive by itself – Example above might be a rare exception to GPR 1 (from Liszt’s Sonata in B minor)

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Local Detail Grouping Preference Rules GPR 2 (Proximity): “Consider a sequence of four notes n 1, n 2, n 3, n 4. All else being equal, the transition n 2 -n 3 may be heard as a group boundary if – a. (Slur/Rest) the interval of time from the end of n 2 to the beginning of n 3 is greater than that from the end of n 1 to the beginning of n 2 and that from the end of n 3 to the beginning of n 4, or if – b. (Attack-Point) the interval of time between the attack points of n 2 and n 3 is greater than that between the attack points of n 1 and n 2 and that between the attack points of n 3 and n 4.” This is L&J’s version of the Gestalt principle of proximity GPR 2 does NOT apply in the cases below. Why?

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Local Detail Grouping Preference Rules GPR 3 (Change): “Consider a sequence of four notes n 1, n 2, n 3, n 4. All else being equal, the transition n 2 -n 3 may be heard as a group boundary if – a. (Register) the transition n 2 -n 3 involves a greater intervallic distance than both n 1 -n 2 and n 3 -n 4, or if – b. (Dynamics) the transition n 2 -n 3 involves a change in dynamics and n 1 -n 2 and n 3 -n 4 do not, or if – c. (Articulation) the transition n 2 -n 3 involves a change in articulation and n 1 -n 2 and n 3 -n 4 do not, or if – d. (Length) n 2 and n 3 are of different lengths and both pairs n 1, n 2 and n 3, n 4 do not differ in length.” “One might add further cases to deal with such things as change in timbre or instrumentation.” (p.46) GPR 3 does not predict any grouping boundaries in the examples below GPR 3a GPR 3bGPR 3cGPR 3d

Applying the local detail rules All places where GPRs 1-3 apply are indeed group boundaries, except – transition from 8 to 9 – transition from 9 to 10 do not hear because is more salient and both would lead to a 1-note group (conflicting with GPR 1) – transition from 18 to 19

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Larger-level grouping rules GPR 4 (Intensification): “Where the effects picked out by GPRs 2 and 3 are relatively more pronounced, a larger-level group boundary may be placed.” (p.50) – In example above, GPR 2 predicts boundaries between each set of triplets – Gap between 3 rd and 4 th set of triplets much bigger, so GPR 4 predicts a higher-level boundary here

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Larger-level grouping rules GPR 5 (Symmetry): “Prefer grouping analyses that most closely approach the ideal subdivision of groups into two parts of equal length.” (p.49) – GPR 5 applies in example above between notes 10 and 11 – Also operates at 6-7 and 16-17

Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Larger-level grouping rules GPR 6 (Parallelism): “Where two or more segments of music can be construed as parallel, they preferably form parallel parts of groups.” (p.51) – Any two groups that begin in a similar way can be construed as parallel (hence “parts of groups”) e.g., bars 1-2, 3-4, 5-6 in example above from Beethoven’s Quartet Op.18, No.1

Deliège’s (1987) Experiments on L&J’s grouping theory: Experiment 1 Deliège carried out two experiments on L&J’s grouping theory In Experiment 1, subjects listened to extracts from instrumental works and marked perceived groups on paper Allowed to listen to examples as many times as necessary – number of times noted by experimenter Compared subjects’ analyses with those predicted by GPRs 2 and 3

Deliège’s (1987) Experiments on L&J’s grouping theory: Experiment 1 Results Results of experiment 1 were: – Both musicians and non-musicians were significantly in agreement with rules – Musicians were significantly more in agreement with rules than non-musicians – Non-musicians did not need significantly more repetitions than musicians – All subjects needed significantly more repetitions when responses not in agreement with rules

Deliège’s (1987) Experiments on L&J’s grouping theory: Experiment 1 Results Recall that L&J suggested addition of further part to GPR3 to account for change in timbre Deliège found three rules to be particularly important – GPR 2b, Attack-Point – GPR 3b, Change in dynamics – a version of GPR 3 for change in timbre Other rules used less by everyone, but much less by non- musicians Results imply that all subjects use primarily the relative lengths of events and changes in loudness and timbre to infer grouping structure Musical training makes people sensitive to other cues too

Deliège (1987): GPR 3d or GPR 2b? GPR 3d predicts a group boundary between end of sequence of short notes and start of sequence of longer notes But listener only knows first long note is first in a sequence of long notes when he hears the beginning of the 3 rd long note GPR 3d therefore implies that listener retrospectively hears a group boundary before the first long note when he hears the 3 rd long note Deliège actually found listeners hear a boundary after the first long note – Seems to be a modified form of GPR 2b

Deliège’s second experiment L&J do not provide any measure of rule strength to allow for resolution of conflict between preference rules Deliège presented subjects with scales modified to investigate effects of GPRS conflicting with each other Listeners were forced to choose one group boundary when rules predicted two (see example above) Found 93% of non-musicians and 97% musicians responded in accordance with rules (i.e., chose one of the two predicted boundaries) Musicians responded significantly more in agreement with rules Change in timbre was the most frequently used cue

References Deliège, I. (1987). Grouping conditions in listening to music: An approach to Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s Grouping Preference Rules. Music Perception, 4(4), 325–360. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. Harcourt, Brace and World, New York. Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt Psychology. Liveright, New York. Lerdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Wertheimer, M. (1938). Laws of organization in perceptual forms. In W. D. Ellis, editor, A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. Routledge and Keegan Paul, London. Originally published in 1923.