What “Mice Trap” tells us about the mental lexicon Carolyn J. Buck-Gengler 1,3, Lise Menn 2,3, and Alice F. Healy 1,3 University of Colorado at Boulder.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Grammar: Meaning and Contexts * From Presentation at NCTE annual conference in Pittsburgh, 2005.
Advertisements

DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY U N I V E R S I T Y O F C O P E N H A G E N Suppression of neutral but not emotional words Background Anderson & Green (2001)
Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination Jessica Maye, Janet F. Werker, LouAnn Gerken A brief article from Cognition.
Processing Multiple Unrelated Meanings versus Multiple Related Senses Ekaterini Klepousniotou McGill University.
1 Language and kids Linguistics lecture #8 November 21, 2006.
Nonword Repetition and Sentence Repetition as Clinical Markers of SLI: The Case of Cantonese Stokes, F. S., Wong, M.Y.A., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, B. L.
Do Children Pick and Choose? An Examination of Phonological Selection and Avoidance in Early Lexical Acquisition. Richard G. Schwartz and Laurence B. Leonard.
The truth shall make you slow: Superlative Quantifiers as speech act modifiers Aviya Hacohen, Dana Kozlowski & Ariel Cohen Ben-Gurion University of the.
The Linguistics of SLA.
Experiment 2: MEG Study Materials and Methods: 11 right-handed subjects with 20:20 vision were run. 3 subjects’ data was discarded because of poor performance.
Phonetic Similarity Effects in Masked Priming Marja-Liisa Mailend 1, Edwin Maas 1, & Kenneth I. Forster 2 1 Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing.
Putting Together the Pieces: Meaning Matters in Children’s Plural Comprehension Craig Van Pay, Areanna Lakowske & Jennifer Zapf.
Running Records.
Newly-Learned Stimuli: The Effects on Lateralized Lexical Decision Travellia Tjokro & Christine Chiarello University of California, Riverside Introduction.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Acquisition: Morphology.
1 Attention and Inhibition in Bilingual Children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort Task By: Ellen Bialystok and Michelle M.Martin.
Word Retrieval in a Stem Completion Task: Influence of Number of Potential Responses Christine Chiarello 1, Laura K. Halderman 1, Cathy S. Robinson 1 &
Propose but Verify: Fast Mapping meets Cross-Situational Learning Medina, Tamara N., Hafri, Alon, Trueswell, John, & Gleitman, Lila R. Background Several.
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 2: Language processing: speed and flexibility.
Discrimination-Shift Problems Background This type of task has been used to compare concept learning across species as well as across a broad range of.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Acquisition: Bilinugalism.
1 Representing Regularity: The English Past Tense Matt Davis William Marslen-Wilson Centre for Speech and Language Birkbeck College University of London.

The study of the structure of words.  Words are an integral part of language ◦ Vocabulary is a dynamic system  How many words do we know? ◦ Infinite.
指導教授:鍾榮富教授 Reporter:NA1C0008陳慧齡
Learning the passive in natural(istic) settings Katie Alcock, Ken Rimba, Manizha Tellaie, and Charles Newton Thanks to Kamil ud Deen.
Students’ Understanding of Human Nature: An Analogical Approach R. BROCK FROST AND ERIC AMSEL Weber State University Introduction University students enter.
Experimental study of morphological priming: evidence from Russian verbal inflection Tatiana Svistunova Elizaveta Gazeeva Tatiana Chernigovskaya St. Petersburg.
Jelena Mirković and Maryellen C. MacDonald Language and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of Wisconsin-Madison Introduction How to Study Subject-Verb.
X Language Acquisition
Background Infants and toddlers have detailed representations for their known vocabulary items Consonants (e.g., Swingley & Aslin, 2000; Fennel & Werker,
Chapter 9: Language and Communication. Chapter 9: Language and Communication Chapter 9 has four modules: Module 9.1 The Road to Speech Module 9.2 Learning.
Introduction Pinker and colleagues (Pinker & Ullman, 2002) have argued that morphologically irregular verbs must be stored as full forms in the mental.
Main Topics  Abstract Analysis:  When Underlying Representations ≠ Surface Forms  Valid motivations/evidence or limits for Abstract Analysis  Empirical.
CHAPTER 10 – VOCABULARY: STUDENTS IN CHARGE Presenter: 1.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Learning Words and Rules Abstract Knowledge of Word Order in Early Sentence Comprehension Yael Gertner.

1. Background Evidence of phonetic perception during the first year of life: from language-universal listeners to native listeners: Consonants and vowels:
LEXICAL LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION IN CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME Abstract LEXICAL LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION IN CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME Elbouz M.
Language and Communication Definitions Developmental scales Communication disorders Speech Disorders Language Disorders Interventions.
CHAPTER 10 – VOCABULARY: STUDENTS IN CHARGE Presenter: Laura Mizuha 1.
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 11 – Language Structure June 2, 2003.
INTRODUCTION : DESCRIBING AND EXPLAINING L2 ACQUISITION Ellis 2003, Chapter 1 PP By. Annisa Rizqi Handayani.
Lexical and morphosyntactic minimal pairs. Evidence for different processing Luca Cilibrasi, Vesna Stojanovik, Patricia Riddell, School of Psychology,
Language and Communication Definitions Developmental scales Communication disorders Speech Disorders Language Disorders Interventions.
Sahin, Pinker, Cash, Schomer, & Halgren (2009) Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area.
REFERENCES Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Troetschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit.
PET Count  Word Frequency effects (coefficients) were reliably related to activation in both the striate and ITG for older adults only.  For older adults,
Grammar Chapter 10. What is Grammar? Basic Points description of patterns speakers use to construct sentences stronger patterns - most nouns form plurals.
Chapter 11 Language. Some Questions to Consider How do we understand individual words, and how are words combined to create sentences? How can we understand.
Introduction Method Experiment 2 In spoken word recognition, phonological and indexical properties (i.e., characteristics of the speaker’s voice) of a.
Charlton Kings Junior School INFORMATION EVENING FOR YEAR 6 PARENTS.
Chapter 10 Language acquisition Language acquisition----refers to the child’s acquisition of his mother tongue, i.e. how the child comes to understand.
Gaze cues in mother-child dyads Heather Bell and Meredith Meyer University of Oregon INTRODUCTION RESULTS CONCLUSIONS METHODS REFERENCES ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.
The Prevalence of Irregular Conjugations as Regular in Spanish Second Language Learners at Miami University Alice Kitrys Abstract.
Constraints on definite article alternation in speech production: To “thee” or not to “thee”? By M. GARETH GASKELL, HELEN COX, KATHERINE FOLEY, HELEN GRIEVE,
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Effects of Reading on Word Learning
Pre-Kindergarten Scope & Sequence Unit 8: Spring is in the Air
Semantic Priming Effects in a Bilingual Gujarati Speaker
عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد
Cognitive Processes in SLL and Bilinguals:
2nd Language Learning Chapter 2 Lecture 4.
Grammar Workshop Thursday 9th June.
Saidna Zulfiqar bin Tahir STATE UNIVERSITY OF MAKASSAR
Young Children’s Reasoning about Gender: Stereotypes or Essences?
Noriko Hoshino Department of Psychology
36.1 – Describe the structural components of language.
Rebecca Baxter – LETS Go! UK Down Syndrome Education International
Form Classes Ed McCorduck English 402—Grammar SUNY Cortland
Presentation transcript:

What “Mice Trap” tells us about the mental lexicon Carolyn J. Buck-Gengler 1,3, Lise Menn 2,3, and Alice F. Healy 1,3 University of Colorado at Boulder 1 Department of Psychology 2 Department of Linguistics 3 Institute of Cognitive Science Abstract Two experiments focused on the effect of noun regularity on the task of producing a singular form from a plural or the reverse. Experiment 1 used English; Experiment 2 used a miniature artificial language in which word frequency and form were controlled. Subjects learned the words in the artificial language before proceeding to the test phase of Experiment 2. In the test phase of both experiments, subjects were given one of the two forms (singular or plural) and required to produce either the same form or the opposite form. In half the trials the grammatical number of the picture and the required response matched, and in half the number did not match. In Experiment 1, response time per letter was faster when the number of cue and required response matched than when they differed, especially for the irregulars. In Experiment 2, there were also significant effects of matching, but these effects were not influenced by either word frequency or regularity. From these results we conclude that the preference for irregular plural as first element of noun-noun compounds can be explained by processing factors which hold for both children and adults in both natural and artificial languages. No appeal to innate grammar is required to explain the similarity between this aspect of child and adult linguistic behavior. Background Regular and irregular nouns behave differently Example: noun-noun compounding in English regularirregular singularrat catchermouse catcher plural*rats catchermice catcher General rule: The first noun must be singular (e.g., rat catcher, toy box, but not *rats catcher, *toys box, even when talking about multiple rats or toys), but the plural of irregular nouns is also acceptable. Pinker (e.g., 1994: 146-7) argues that the rule is likely to be innate because children obey it (Gordon, 1985), even without being exposed to adult examples of irregular plurals as first elements of compounds. Gordon (1985): When prompted with phrases like “What do you call someone who eats ?”, where X was either a regular or irregular plural noun, children frequently used plural when primed with an irregular plural, but almost never used plural when primed with a regular plural. Findings were explained in terms of an innate grammar, specifically Level Ordering (e.g., Kiparsky, 1982). Alternate explanation: Processing difficulty Buck-Gengler, Menn, and Healy (2001): Elicited noun-noun compounds (similar to Gordon, 1985) from adults Subjects saw fill-in-the-blank sentence, responded with compound a TUB holding X is a _____ X was a singular or plural regular or irregular English noun Example: duck/ducks/goose/geese Subjects produced far more plurals for the first noun of the compound when the cue was an irregular plural (geese) than when it was a regular plural (ducks) or either type of singular (goose/duck). When subjects did produce singular first forms (goose tub/duck tub) RTs were significantly longer for irregular plural cues than for the other three kinds of cue. Preference for producing irregular plurals as first elements of compounds in such elicitation tasks can therefore be explained by processing difficulty: Goose is harder to access from geese than duck is from ducks (as might be predicted by, e.g., Allen & Badecker, 2002, or Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999). __________________________________ Acknowledgments This research was supported in part by Army Research Institute Contract DASW01-99-K and Army Research Office Grant DAAG to the University of Colorado (Alice Healy, Principal Investigator; Lyle Bourne, Co-Principal Investigator). This research was also supported in part by a Student Research Award from the Institute of Cognitive Science to the first author. The Current Experiments Main Task: Given either a singular or a plural of a noun, produce either same or opposite form Experiment 1: English – similar set of words as in Buck-Gengler et al. (2001) Stimuli: Pictures representing a matched set of imageable English nouns IrregularRegular childchildrencarcars footfeetforkforks goosegeesegunguns louseliceletterletters manmenmatchmatches mousemicemoonmoons oxoxenowlowls toothteethtreetrees Matched on frequency, first letter, length Experiment 2: Miniature artificial language Why an artificial language? To control for factors in real languages (e.g., word frequency, type of irregularity, aspects of form including onset, length, etc., and other idiosyncrasies) that could be contributing to the response. Description of artificial language: Structure -- controlled for form and frequency Form singularplural CVCV C1 C2, V1 V2Plural method C = {k,g,f,v,m,n,s,z,t,d,p,b} Noun typePrefixInfix V = {a,i,o,u}Regulare-CVCVC-e-VCV IrregularC-#-VCV#-CVCV # is a nonpredictable vowel (not e) Frequency 12 low frequency regular4 low frequency irregular 4 high frequency regular4 high frequency irregular Stimuli: Pictures representing the English concept for each artificial language word Learning the Miniature Artificial Language Subjects saw a picture and associated word, and typed the word 12 words (mixture of high and low frequency, singular and plural, regular and irregular) formed a subset After each subset, quiz to reinforce learning, with feedback 8 subsets comprised one round; within the 8 subsets each word was seen either once (low frequency) or 4 times (high frequency) After one round of learning and quizzes, test of all words, with feedback When criterion (90%) reached on test, proceeded to main task If criterion not reached, repeated learn/quiz, with different order and grouping of words  All Ss learned to criterion within 7 rounds; over half learned to criterion in 2 or 3 rounds. Method Both experiments: 2 phases Phase 1 Exp. 1: familiarization with pictures see picture, type word; go through list twice Exp. 2: Learn miniature artificial language words Phase 2 Main task Participants and Apparatus: 16 (E1), 24 (E2) native speakers of English Presented on computer Responses and RTs recorded by computer Main task: In both experiments, participants saw a picture and a fill-in-the-blank phrase which was either the number “1” or the number “4” followed by a blank (in the English experiment the numbers were spelled out). Example fill-in-the-blank phrases: “one _____” or “four ____” (Exp. 1) “1_____” or “4 ______” (Exp. 2) Participants typed the appropriate form of the English or artificial language word representing the picture to go with the number in the fill in the blank phrase. For example: If they saw a picture of four trees and “one ____” (Exp. 1) or “1 ____” (Exp. 2) they responded by typing “tree” (Exp. 1) or “bidu” (Exp. 2) Design: Between subjects factor: (Exp. 2) Plural method of regular (prefix; infix) Within subjects factors: Noun type (regular, irregular) Grammatical number of response (singular, plural) Match (Response grammatical number match/not match cue picture) Frequency (high, low) (Exp. 2 only) Measure: Time/letter (Total Response Time/# letters in the word) Scoring: Only correct typed trials (no backspacing/correction) included __________________________________ References Allen, M. & Badecker, W. (2002). Inflectional regularity: Probing the nature of lexical representation in a cross-modal priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 705–722. Buck-Gengler, C. J., Menn, L., & Healy, A. F. (2001). Mice Trap: A new explanation for irregular plurals in noun-noun compounds. Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gordon, P. (1985). Level-ordering in lexical development. Cognition, 21(2), Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. In H. v. d. Hulst & N. Smith (Eds.), The Structure of Phonological Representations (pp ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris Publications. Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75. Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: W. Morrow and Co. Results Exp. 1 (English): Main effects for Response Time/Letter: Regulars faster than irregulars Additional time is required when the picture number and response number don’t match Interactions: Exp. 2 (Miniature Artificial Language): Main effects for Response Time/Letter: Regulars faster than irregulars Additional time is required when the picture number and response number don’t match Conclusion From these and previous results we conclude that the frequently reported preference for irregular plural as first element of noun-noun compounds can be explained by processing factors (i.e., the accessibility of the singular from the plural) that hold for both children and adults, and in both natural and artificial languages. No appeal to innate grammar is required to explain the similarity between child and adult performance on this aspect of linguistic behavior. More generally, the closer two forms of the same word (lemma) are to each other, the faster one will be accessed from the other. Unlike English, the artificial language ensured that the difference in form between singular and plural was equivalent for both regulars and irregulars. With this control, we ’ ve teased apart the issues of regularity and similarity, and have shown them to have separate, non-interacting effects. In addition, infixation and affixation appear to be equivalent when transparency is controlled. Letters in irregular singulars are typed faster than those in irregular plurals, but letters in regular plurals are typed faster than those in regular singulars. Shows the two main effects; moreover, the interaction approaches significance: Extra response time is needed when cue and response numbers don’t match for irregular nouns. Shows the two main effects (interaction is not significant).