HW#9: read Chapter 2.6 pages 154-157 On page 159 #2.122, page 160#2.124, 2.126.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Question of Causation YMS3e 4.3:Establishing Causation AP Statistics Mr. Molesky.
Advertisements

Correlation and Causation Part III – Causation. This video is designed to accompany pages in Making Sense of Uncertainty Activities for Teaching.
O BSERVATIONAL S TUDY VS. E XPERIMENT. O BSERVATIONAL S TUDY The researcher observes individuals and measures variables of interest without influencing.
CHAPTER 6.1 Part 1. REVIEW  Observational Study  Observe  Record  Measure  What is the difference between an observational study and an experiment?
Aim: How do we establish causation?
AP Statistics Section 4.3 Establishing Causation
AP Statistics Causation & Relations in Categorical Data.
Correlation: Relationships Can Be Deceiving. The Impact Outliers Have on Correlation An outlier that is consistent with the trend of the rest of the data.
Correlation: Relationships Can Be Deceiving. An outlier is a data point that does not fit the overall trend. Speculate on what influence outliers have.
Lesson Establishing Causation. Knowledge Objectives Identify the three ways in which the association between two variables can be explained. Define.
AP STATS: 50 point quiz Sit with your partner. This is open notes/textbook. Work for 20 minutes with your partner on the quiz. Each person will have to.
Experiments and Observational Studies.  A study at a high school in California compared academic performance of music students with that of non-music.
 Pg : 3b, 6b (form and strength)  Page : 10b, 12a, 16c, 16e.
Chapter 4 Section 3 Establishing Causation
The Question of Causation
1 10. Causality and Correlation ECON 251 Research Methods.
 Correlation and regression are closely connected; however correlation does not require you to choose an explanatory variable and regression does. 
C HAPTER 4: M ORE ON T WO V ARIABLE D ATA Sec. 4.2 – Cautions about Correlation and Regression.
CHAPTER 6- DESIGNING EXPERIMENTS. SECTION 6.1 Experiment vs. Observational Study (again!) * Experiment = treatment applied to all subjects * Obs. Study.
The Practice of Statistics Third Edition Chapter 4: More about Relationships between Two Variables Copyright © 2008 by W. H. Freeman & Company Daniel S.
1 Chapter 4: More on Two-Variable Data 4.1Transforming Relationships 4.2Cautions 4.3Relations in Categorical Data.
Warm Up I. Pilots or Senators: Which team has played better? vs. East vs. West vs. East vs. West WINSLOSSES PCT. WINS LOSSES PCT. WINSLOSSES PCT. WINS.
1 Chapter 4: More on Two-Variable Data 4.1Transforming Relationships 4.2Cautions 4.3Relations in Categorical Data.
4.3: Establishing Causation Both correlation and regression are very useful in describing the relationship between two variables; however, they are first.
Relationships Can Be Deceiving Statistics lecture 5.
+ The Practice of Statistics, 4 th edition – For AP* STARNES, YATES, MOORE Chapter 4: Designing Studies Section 4.2 Experiments.
 Get out your homework and materials for notes!  Take-home quiz due!
Lecture PowerPoint Slides Basic Practice of Statistics 7 th Edition.
Does Association Imply Causation? Sometimes, but not always! What about: –x=mother's BMI, y=daughter's BMI –x=amt. of saccharin in a rat's diet, y=# of.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data: Experiments. Chapter 9 Concepts 2  Observation vs. Experiment  Subjects, Factors, Treatments  How to Experiment Badly 
1 Chapter 4: More on Two-Variable Data 4.1Transforming Relationships 4.2Cautions 4.3Relations in Categorical Data.
AP STATISTICS LESSON 4 – 2 ( DAY 1 ) Cautions About Correlation and Regression.
Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.
Describing Relationships
Cautions About Correlation and Regression Section 4.2.
Copyright © 2013, 2010 and 2007 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter Describing the Relation between Two Variables 4.
Chapter 5 EPS Due 10/30/15 42 Points.
The Question of Causation
Section Causation AP Statistics ww.toddfadoir.com/apstats.
Experimental Designs Section 7.5. Variables Explanatory variable Response Variable (Independent Variable)(Dependent Variable) "Explains observed outcomes""Measures.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
Prediction and Causation How do we predict a response? Explanatory Variables can be used to predict a response: 1. Prediction is based on fitting a line.
Data Analysis Causation Goal: I can distinguish between correlation and causation. (S-ID.9)
Experiments Textbook 4.2. Observational Study vs. Experiment Observational Studies observes individuals and measures variables of interest, but does not.
Analytical Thinking What This Course Is About: Using Elements Of Social Science To Critically Examine American Society. Goal: Becoming “American Mythbusters”
The Question of Causation 4.2:Establishing Causation AP Statistics.
AP Statistics. Issues Interpreting Correlation and Regression  Limitations for r, r 2, and LSRL :  Can only be used to describe linear relationships.
2.7 The Question of Causation
Cautions About Correlation and Regression Section 4.2
Cautions About Correlation and Regression
Proving Causation Why do you think it was me?!.
Establishing Causation
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Cautions about Correlation and Regression
Section 4.3 Types of Association
Chapter 2 Looking at Data— Relationships
Register for AP Exams --- now there’s a $10 late fee per exam
7.2 Interpreting Correlations
Cautions about Correlation and Regression
7 Minutes of Silence Determine if the data is linear or exponential.
The Question of Causation
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Statistical Reasoning December 8, 2015 Chapter 6.2
Least-Squares Regression
EQ: What gets in the way of a good model?
Does Association Imply Causation?
4.2 Cautions about Correlation and Regression
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Section 6.2 Establishing Causation
Chapter 4: More on Two-Variable Data
Presentation transcript:

HW#9: read Chapter 2.6 pages On page 159 #2.122, page 160#2.124, 2.126

The Question of Causation When looking at the relationship between two variables, the goal is to establish that changes in the explanatory variable (independent variable) CAUSES changes in the response variable (dependent variable) Even when a strong association is present, the conclusion that this association is due to a causal link between the variables is often hard to find.

Examples of observed associations 1. Mother’s body mass index and daughter's body mass index 2. Amount of artificial sweetener saccharin in a rat’s diet and count of tumors in the rat’s bladder 3. A student’s SAT scores as a high school senior and a student’s first-year college grade point average 4. Monthly flow of money into stock mutual funds and monthly rate of return for the stock market

Explaining association

Examples of direct causation 1. Mother’s body mass index and daughter's body mass index 2. Amount of artificial sweetener saccharin in a rat’s diet and count of tumors in the rat’s bladder  The only ones with also have direct causation

Explaining association: CAUSATION A study of Mexican American girls aged 9 to 12 years recorded body mass index (BMI), a measure of weight relative to height, for both the girls and their mothers. People with high BMI are overweight or obese. The study also measured hours of television, minutes of physical activity, and intake of several kinds of food. The strongest correlation (r = 0.506) was between the BMI of daughters and the BMI of their mothers. Body type is in part determined by heredity. Daughters inherit half their genes from their mothers. There is therefore a direct causal link between the BMI of mothers and daughters. Yet the mothers' BMIs explain only 25.6% (that's r 2 again) of the variation among the daughters' BMIs. Other factors, such as diet and exercise, also influence BMI. Even when direct causation is present, it is rarely a complete explanation of an association between two variables.

Best evidence of causation Comes from experiments that actually change x while holding all other factors fixed; therefore if y changes, there is a good reason to think that x caused the change in y Example: Experiments show that large amounts of saccharin—about 5% of the entire diet—cause bladder tumors in rats. Should we avoid saccharin as a replacement for sugar in food? Rats are not people. Although we can't experiment with people, studies of people who consume different amounts of saccharin fail to find an association between saccharin and bladder tumors. Even well-established causal relations may not generalize to other settings.

Explaining Association: COMMON RESPONSE The observed association between x and y is explained by a lurking variable z Both x and y change in response to changes in z This common response creates an association even though there may be no direct causal link between x and y

Example of Common Response 1. A student’s SAT scores as a high school senior and a student’s first-year college grade point average 2. Monthly flow of money into stock mutual funds and monthly rate of return for the stock market  Examples of how common response can create an association

Explaining Association: COMMON RESPONSE Example 1: SAT scores and college grades. Students who are smart and who have learned a lot tend to have both high SAT scores and high college grades. The positive correlation is explained by this common response to students' ability and knowledge. Example 2: Stock market performance and investments in mutual funds. There is a strong positive correlation between how much money individuals add to mutual funds each month and how well the stock market does the same month. Is the new money driving the market up? The correlation may be explained in part by common response to underlying investor sentiment: when optimism reigns, individuals send money to funds and large institutions also invest more. The institutions would drive up prices even if individuals did nothing. In addition, what causation there is may operate in the other direction: when the market is doing well, individuals rush to add money to their mutual funds.

Explaining Association: Confounding Two variables are confounding when their effects on a response variable cannot be distinguished from each other. The confounding variable may be either explanatory variables or lurking variables. When many variables interact with each other, confounding of several variables often prevent us from drawing conclusions about causation Because x is confounded with z, we cannot distinguish the influence of x from the influence of z We cannot say how strong the direct effect of x on y is; it can be hard to say if x influences y at all

Examples of Confounding 1. Whether a person regularly attends religious services and how long the person lives 2. The number of years of education a worker has and the worker’s income  Associations are explained in part by confounding

Explaining Association: Confounding Example 1: Religion and a long life. Many studies have found that people who are active in their religion live longer than nonreligious people. But people who attend church or mosque or synagogue also take better care of themselves than nonattenders. They are less likely to smoke, more likely to exercise, and less likely to be overweight. The effects of these good habits are confounded with the direct effects of attending religious services. Example 2: Education and income. It is likely that more education is a cause of higher income—many highly paid professions require advanced education. However, confounding is also present. People who have high ability and come from prosperous homes are more likely to get many years of education than people who are less able or poorer. Of course, people who start out able and rich are more likely to have high earnings even without much education. We can't say how much of the higher income of well-educated people is actually caused by their education.

Confounding and Common Response Both common response and confounding involve the influences of a lurking variable (or variables) z on the response variable y The distribution between these two types of relationships is less important than the common element, the influence of lurking variables *EVEN A VERY STRONG ASSOCIATION BETWEEN TWO VAIRABLES IS NOT BY ITSELF GOOD EVIDENCE THAT THERE IS A CAUSE-AND-EFFECT LINK BETWEEN THE VARIABLES*

Homework: Online courses. Many colleges offer online versions of some courses that are also taught in the classroom. It often happens that the students who enroll in the online version do better than the classroom students on the course exams. This does not show that online instruction is more effective than classroom teaching, because the kind of people who sign up for online courses are often quite different from the classroom students. Suggest some student characteristics that you think could be confounded with online versus classroom. Use a diagram like Figure 2.28(c) (page 154) to illustrate your ideas.Figure 2.28page 154