Ecological Determinants of School Readiness: A Longitudinal Study Mark Wade, Dillon Browne, Heather Prime, & Jennifer Jenkins Department of Human Development.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Within families: family-wide and child-specific influences on childrens socio-emotional development Jennifer Jenkins, Jon Rasbash, Tom OConnor.
Advertisements

Shared, child-specific and reciprocal influences in the development of psychopathology Jenny Jenkins, Judy Dunn, Jon Rasbash, Tom OConnor, Anna Simpson,
3 High expectations for every child
Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
Large databases vs. individual analysis: Two complimentary approaches in the study of education and learning Esther Adi-Japha School of Education, Bar-Ilan.
Children and Poverty McLoyd (1998) Childhood poverty is a major problem in the US –Over 22% of children in the US live in poverty as compared to 9% in.
Domestic Violence, Parenting, and Behavior Outcomes of Children Chien-Chung Huang Rutgers University.
Principles of Developmental Assessment PSY 417. Assessment Zero to Three Work Group: “Process designed to deepen an understanding of a child’s competencies.
Abstract Investigated the mother-child relationship and adolescents’ emotional symptoms and adaptive skills reported by youth. Negative mother-child relationship.
The Social Consequences of Economic Inequality for Canadian Children: A Review of the Canadian Literature.
Association Between Socioeconomic Risk and Parenting Behaviours Association Between Socioeconomic Risk and Parenting Behaviours Nicole Gridley, Judy Hutchings.
Marital Satisfaction and Family Functioning in Families with Toddlers: Evidence For a Single Construct? Phillip R. Sevigny, M. A. & Lynn Loutzenhiser,
1 Christina Rinaldi, PhD, RPsych University of Alberta Research funded by the Alberta Centre for Child, Family & Community Research.
The Achievement Gap: Lessons from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study – Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) Tamara Halle, Nicole Forry, Elizabeth Hair & Kate Perper.
Introduction Research Question ▪ Approximately 13% of mothers and fathers in the community develop clinical levels of depressive symptoms after the birth.
Income-related gaps in school readiness in the U.S. and the U.K. Jane Waldfogel Elizabeth Washbrook Child Well-Being and Social Investments in the U.S.
The Influence of Parent Education on Child Outcomes: The Mediating Role of Parents Beliefs and Behaviors Pamela E. Davis-Kean University of Michigan This.
Social Competence and Adjustment in Chinese and North American Children: A Contextual-Developmental Perspective Xinyin Chen University of Western Ontario.
Participants Year 7 data from the Ontario Looking After Children (OnLAC) project 1,063 children between the ages of years of age 459 girls and 604.
V v Do Older Parents have More Self-Regulated Children? Kevin G. Harper, Megan M. McClelland, Ph.D. Oregon State University COLLEGE OF PUBLIC HEALTH AND.
Educational Psychology Part III Draw and discuss the significant aspects of the model of the teaching/learning process presented in class (or discussed.
Understanding The Early Years Niagara College ECE Program  October 2007 Glory Ressler, B.A., Dip. GIT Coordinator, Understanding the Early Years Niagara.
Researcher Perspective Talk: Modelling developmental processes Vaso Totsika CEDAR.
LITERATURE REVIEW Lilian Bodunrin “The Effects of a Single-Parent Home on an African American Child’s Education” Texas Tech University.
Ramey & Ramey (1998) Early Intervention: activities designed to enhance a young child’s development Initial evaluation of child’s abilities and needs (in.
Links to Positive Parenting among African American and Hispanic American Low-Income Mothers Laura D. Pittman Psychology Department Northern Illinois University.
Acknowledgments: Data for this study were collected as part of the CIHR Team: GO4KIDDS: Great Outcomes for Kids Impacted by Severe Developmental Disabilities.
Birth Cohort Jennifer Park National Center for Education Statistics Institute of Education Sciences IES Research Conference June 2006.
2012 State of the Science Congress on Nursing Research Jeanne Alhusen 1, Deborah Gross 1, Matthew Hayat 2, and Phyllis Sharps 1 1 Johns Hopkins University.
Week 4: Multiple regression analysis Overview Questions from last week What is regression analysis? The mathematical model Interpreting the β coefficient.
Disentangling the Relations between Discrimination, Cultural Orientation, Social Support, and Coping in Mexican American Adolescents Megan O’Donnell Mark.
Differential Parenting as a Predictor of Child Psychopathology Courtney Ficks, Whitney Guthrie, Lisa Doelger, Karina R. Horowitz, & Kathryn Lemery-Chalfant.
Growing Up and Moving On: Family Involvement in Transition Lauren Lindstrom, Ph.D. University of Oregon Youth Transition Program Conference February 16,
Maternal Romantic Relationship Quality, Parenting Stress and Child Outcomes: A Mediational Model Christine R. Keeports, Nicole J. Holmberg, & Laura D.
Inequalities in Children’s Educational Outcomes: Using Administrative Data to Gain a Population-Based Perspective on Health Marni Brownell, Noralou Roos,
Acknowledgments: Data for this study were collected as part of the CIHR Team: GO4KIDDS: Great Outcomes for Kids Impacted by Severe Developmental Disabilities.
Can Peer Pressure Be A Good Thing? Megan M. Schad, Meredyth A. Evans, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
Longitudinal Links between Neighborhood Problems, Collective Efficacy, and Adolescents’ Academic and Socioemotional Outcomes Shay M. Galto, Danielle M.
+ Child & Adolescent Psychology Welcome to Seminar #8.
V v References 1. Ladd, G. W., Birch, H. S., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children's Social and Scholastic Lives in Kindergarten: Related Spheres of Influence?
Independent Associations Between Dimensions of Externalizing Behavior in Toddlerhood and School-Age Academic Achievement Lauretta M. Brennan, Daniel S.
Introduction Early Childhood Characteristics and Academic Achievement  Research has demonstrated continuity between toddler- age language ability and.
Paediatric Language Group  In children
Temperament Constitutionally based individual differences in behavioral characteristics that are relatively consistent across situations and over time.
Childcare Mckim et al., 1999 Studied effects of childcare on attachment Participants: Families with infants between 2 and 30 months Visited homes 2-3 weeks.
Family Characteristics Effect of parental separation on children's behavior 13.8% of children born in experienced parental separation before age.
Contingent parental investment: An evolutionary framework for understanding early interaction between mothers and children David Beaulieu, Daphne Bugental.
Adolescent attachment state-of-mind and parent-adolescent relationship quality: Joint longitudinal predictors of social and emotional functioning Kathleen.
Parental Involvement: A Path to Achievement Christa L. Green, Kathleen V. Hoover-Dempsey, Howard M. Sandler; Vanderbilt University Joan M. T. Walker; Long.
Sociocultural Influences Related to Language, Cognition, and Social Emotional Developmental Relationships Presenters: Kimberly Sharkins & Dr. James Ernest.
Introduction Results: Moderated Mediation The Immigrant Paradox: Associations Between Latino Kindergartners’ Reading and Mathematics Skills, Parents’ Knowledge.
Discussion Results Introduction From Lateral to Leader: A Study of Preschoolers’ Relationships with Peers Erin Podgorski & Dr. Carin L. Neitzel, The University.
Research on the relationship between childhood sleep problems and substance use in adolescents and young adults is limited. This knowledge gap has been.
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
Longitudinal Effects of Mothers’ Encouragement and Discouragement of Positive Emotions on Children’s Task Behavior Deon Brown, Julie C. Dunsmore, Erika.
Indiana Paths to QUALITY™ Child Care Quality Rating and Improvement System: Outcomes for Children and Child Care Providers James Elicker, Zachary Gold,
Preschool-Age, Low-Income, Hispanic Children; Relations Between Parent-Child Interactions and Child Language Development in English and Spanish Reina Izabel.
Texas Pediatric Society Electronic Poster Contest
Predictors of Parenting Self-Efficacy in Parents Attending College
Mothers' Vocabulary and Autonomy-Granting Behaviors as Predictors of Gains in Children's Vocabulary Competence from Age 3 to Age 4 Sara L. Sohr-Preston.
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Reporting the evidence:
Connecting Childhood and Mental Illness
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Child Care and Young Children’s Development
Prosocial Behaviors in Adolescence
A holistic approach to child development: factors influencing overall development in children in care in Ontario, Canada Meagan Millera, Barbara Greenberga,
Korey F. Beckwith & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Study Design/Methods Used
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Presentation transcript:

Ecological Determinants of School Readiness: A Longitudinal Study Mark Wade, Dillon Browne, Heather Prime, & Jennifer Jenkins Department of Human Development & Applied Psychology; University of Toronto Introduction: Background/ Rationale: Children develop within multiple layers of context (i.e. family, school, neighbourhood) which interact with one another to influence child outcomes (e.g. Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Many of these child outcomes are associated with one another. For example, academic achievement is both concurrently (e.g. Nelson et al., 2004) and longitudinally (e.g. Masten et al., 2005) associated with internalizing and externalizing problems. One of the strongest predictors of later academic achievement is school-entry reading and language ability (e.g. Duncan et al., 2007). Thus, in order to promote positive child outcomes, it is important to identify multilevel influences on children’s reading and language ability since these school-readiness skills may predict later academic and non-academic (e.g. emotional, behavioural, etc.) outcomes. Identification of these multilevel risk and resiliency factors may provide targets for intervention and policy development. Past Research Numerous factors are related to children’s school readiness skills. Child-level influences include early language skills (Duncan et al., 2007); Family-level influences include stimulation from the home environment (Elardo et al., 1977) and maternal sensitivity (Tamis- LeMonda et al., 2001); Macro-environmental influences include socioeconomic status (SES) (Bradley et al., 2001), maternal education, and ethnicity (Christian et al., 1998). Also, aspects of neighbourhood (e.g. social cohesion and control) are associated with warm, affectionate, and stimulating parenting, and may thus exert indirect effects on children’s language and reading development. Present Study: While the influence of these multilevel factors on language is well documented, their relationship to reading is less well known. Here, we take factors known to be important for language development to examine effects for school readiness in language (i.e. receptive vocabulary) and reading (i.e. print recognition). 1) Examine the direct effect of neighbourhood (assessed during infancy) on reading and language at 3 years. 2) By examining multilevel influences within a single model, we can elucidate the proportion of variability accounted for by individual- level, family-level, and macro-level factors. This will be useful in making policy decisions about where to allocate effort/resources. 3) By understanding how factors at different levels combine to compensate for one another, we can learn more about how children living in similar circumstances are differentially advantaged. For example, how might a child with poor early language skills show good reading at school entry? Also, is there any way a mother with low education can advantage her child? Methods Participants The sample included 501 families from the Kids, Families, and Places (KFP) study - an ongoing longitudinal study taking place in Toronto and Hamilton. Compared to 2006 Census data, study families were similar on family size, income, immigration status, and marital status. Education levels of KFP families were higher than census families. Data on the youngest children from each family are reported. Data were collected at three times (infancy, 18 months and 3 years) with the use of questionnaires, child testing, taped interactions of mothers with children, interviewer observations, and aggregated census data. Measures Canadian-Born Status a. Mother-report of birthplace. Socioeconomic Status a. Composite of household assets and income, reported by the mother. Neighbourhood Quality a. Composite of social cohesion, trust, and control in the neighbourhood, reported by the mother. Maternal Education a. Years of maternal education, reported by the mother. Child Language b. MacArthur CDI was used as a mother-reported measure of children’s expressive language, standardized for sex and age. Maternal Sensitivity b. 5-minute mother-child interactions were coded using the Coding of Attachment-Related Parenting (CARP; Matias, 2006) measure, and the Parchisy (PAR; Deater-Deckard et al., 1997). Early Reading Ability c. Print recognition assessed with Get Ready To Read Receptive Vocabulary c. Standard scores on the PPVT were used. **Note: a = measured during infancy, b = measured at 1.5 years, c = measured at 3 years There are multilevel influences on children’s language and reading ability at 3 years. These effects are independently small, but when examined together account for an appreciable amount of variability. For reading, there are important contingencies between child-, family-, and macro-level variables: (1) only children with adequate language skills and sensitive mothers show gains in reading at age 3; kids without sensitive mom’s do not show such gains regardless of language level. (2) Neighbourhood quality does not affect the kid’s of mothers who are well educated, whereas kids of less educated mom’s may benefit from living in a better neighbourhood. The latter is in line with results from Christian et al. (1998), who showed that children of less educated mother’s had improved academics when engaged in child care. Our results suggest that neighbourhood quality may also predict better reading in these kids. The positive interactive effect of maternal sensitivity and language on children’s reading suggests that these may be appropriate targets for intervention. In general, policy decisions that focus on family- level influences and mother education may prove most effective. Discussion: Results: References: Step 1: Bivariate associations between all predictor and outcome variables were examined using Pearson’s correlation coefficients. Significant predictors were retained and combined into exploratory multiple regression models. Step 2: A number of common, significant predictor variables of both Early Reading and Receptive Vocabulary were identified and selected. These variables were entered hierarchically in to a regression model as follows: Child-level effects Family-level effects Macro-level effects. The final models for reading and language are presented in Table 1 and Table 2. The total variance predicted by each level is presented in Table 3 and Table 4. Step 3: Several contingency (interaction) effects were also explored. Term B SE β p Intercept Child Language (Age 1.5 years) Maternal Sensitivity (Age 1.5 years) Maternal Education SES Canadian Born Parent Neighbourhood Quality Maternal Sensitivity * Child Language Maternal Education * Neighbourhood Quality Term B SE β p Intercept Child Language (Age 1.5 years) Maternal Sensitivity (Age 1.5 years) Maternal Education SES Canadian Born Parent Neighbourhood Quality Table 1: Final Model for Early Reading AbilityTable 2: Final Model for Receptive Vocabulary Note: Only common, significant predictors of Early Reading Ability and Receptive Vocabulary are included in the final models. Model % of Variance p Child Effects (Language) Family Effects (Mat Sens) Mat Ed + SES Effects CA Born + Neighbourhood Quality Maternal Sensitivity * Child Language Maternal Education * Neighbourhood Quality Model % of Variance p Child Effects (Language) Family Effects (Mat Sens) Mat Ed + SES Effects CA Born + Neighbourhood Quality Figure 1: Early Reading as a function of Maternal Sensitivity and Child Language Figure 2: Early Reading as a function of Maternal Education and Neighbourhood Quality The final model for Receptive Vocabulary accounted for 28% of the variance, R 2 =.28, F (6, 182) = 12.02, p <.001. Table 3: Variance Predicted by Each Level for Early ReadingTable 4: Variance Predicted by Each Level for Vocabulary The final model for Early Reading ability accounted for 23% of the variance, R 2 =.23, F (8, 192) = 6.82, p <.001. Main effects must be interpreted with caution due to the presence of significant interactions; these are plotted in Figure 1 and Figure 2. Results Cont’d… Bradley et al., (2001). The home environments of children in the United States part II: Relations with behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Dev, 72(6). Bronfenbrenner, (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design Harv. Univ. Pr. Duncan et al., (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Dev Psych, 43(6). Elardo et al., (1977). A longitudinal study of the relation of infants' home environments to language development at age three. Child Dev, Masten et al., (2005). Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Dev Psych, 41(5). Nelson et al., (2004). Academic achievement of K-12 students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Excep Child, 71(1) Tamis ‐ LeMonda et al., (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Dev, 72(3).